Skip to main content
Log in

The Pedagogue, the Engineer, and the Friend

From Whom Do We Learn?

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Humans can follow different social learning strategies, sometimes oriented toward the models’ characteristics (i.e., who-strategies). The goal of the present study was to explore which who-strategy is preferentially followed in the technological context based on the models’ psychological characteristics. We identified three potential who-strategies: Copy the pedagogue (a model with high theory-of-mind skills), copy the engineer (a model with high technical-reasoning skills), and copy the friend (a model with high level of prosocialness). We developed a closed-group micro-society paradigm in which participants had to build the highest possible towers. Participants began with an individual building phase. Then, they were gathered to discuss the best solutions to increase tower height. After this discussion phase, they had to make a new building attempt, followed by another discussion phase, and so forth for a total of six building phases and five discussion rounds. This methodology allowed us to create an attraction score for each participant (the more an individual was copied in a group, the greater the attraction score). We also assessed participants’ theory-of-mind skills, technical-reasoning skills, and prosocialness to predict participants’ attraction scores based on these measures. Results show that we learn from engineers (high technical-reasoning skills) because they are the most successful. Their attraction power is not immediate, but after they have been identified as attractors, their technique is copied irrespective of their pedagogy (theory-of-mind skills) or friendliness (prosocialness). These findings open avenues for the study of the cognitive bases of human technological culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bernaud, J. L., Priou, P., & Simonnet, R. (1994). NV7: Batterie Multifactorielle d’Aptitudes. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., & Huyvaert, K. P. (2011). AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: Some background, observations and comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2008). Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 165–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanism and cumulative cultural evolution. Psychological Science, 20, 1478–1487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2010). Human cumulative culture in laboratory: Effects of (micro) population size. Learning and Behavior, 38, 310–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., Renner, E., & Atkinson, M. (2018). Human teaching and cumulative cultural evolution. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9, 751–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., Schillinger, K., Evans, C. L., & Hopper, L. M. (2012). End state copying by humans (Homo sapiens): Implications for a comparative perspective on cumulative culture. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 161–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caprara, G. V., Steca, P., Zelli, A., & Capanna, C. (2005). A new scale for measuring adults’ prosocialness. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Heller, S., Birch, S., & Henrich, J. (2012). Prestige-biased cultural learning: Bystander’s differential attention to potential models influences children’s learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 46–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, J. L., Ouden, D. H. E. M., Heyes, C. M., , & Cools, R. (2014). The social dominance paradox. Current Biology, 24, 2812–2816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Model (NEO-FFI). Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Attention and Performance XXI: Processes of change in brain and cognitive development (pp. 249–274). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Oliveira, E., Reynaud, E., & Osiurak, F. (2019). Roles of technical reasoning, theory of mind, creativity, and fluid cognition in cumulative technological culture. Human Nature, 30, 326–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335, 1114–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M., Beugin, M. P., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503, 389–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M., Bonnefon, J. F., Boyd, R., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Causal understanding is not necessary for the improvement of culturally evolving technology. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 446–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunstone, J., & Caldwell, C. A. (2018). Cumulative culture and explicit metacognition: A review of theories, evidence and key predictions. Palgrave Communications, 4, 145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 56–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fay, N., De Kleine, N., Walker, B., & Caldwell, C. A. (2019). Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 116, 6726–6731.

  • Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83, 911–925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levenson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 229–255). Oxford: Berg Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 366, 1139–1148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2016). Who knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 204–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, ÁV., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Prestige-biased social learning: Current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Communications, 5, 20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, R., Hopper, L. M., Whiten, A., Brosnan, S. F., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Hoppitt, W. (2015). Chimpanzees copy dominant and knowledgeable individuals: Implications for cultural diversity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 65–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M., & Jones, P. L. (2018). Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 651–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., & Laland, K. N. (2009). Adaptive trade-offs in the use of social and personal information. In R. Dukas & J. Ratcliffe (Eds.), Cognitive ecology II (pp. 249–271). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning and Behavior, 32, 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2008). An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: adaptive landscapes, producer-scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 350–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2008). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 73, 3–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & Thornton, A. (2018). What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 285, 20180712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 3489–3501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miton, H., & Charbonneau, M. (2018). Cumulative culture in the laboratory: Methodological and theoretical changes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 285, 20180677.

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 639–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2012). The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 279, 653–662.

  • Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., Cross, C. P., Evans, C., Kearney, R., de la Torre, I., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6, 6029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthukrishna, M., Shulman, B. W., Vasilescu, V., & Henrich, J. (2014). Sociality influences cultural complexity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20132511.

  • Orban, G. A., & Caruana, F. (2014). The neural basis of human tool use. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F. (2014). What neuropsychology tells us about human tool use? The four constraints theory (4CT): Mechanics, space, time and effort. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 88–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychological Review, 123, 534–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., De Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., Lesourd, M., Cladière, N., & Reynaud, E. (2016). Physical intelligence does matter to cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 941–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Heinke, D. (2018). Looking for Intoolligence: A unified framework for the cognitive study of human tool use and technology. American Psychologist, 73, 169–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., De Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020a). The castaway island: Distinct roles of theory of mind and technical reasoning in cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149, 158–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020b). Technition: When tools come out of the closet. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 880–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2020). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ottoni, E. B., de Resende, B. D., & Izar, P. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others’ tool-using skills. Animal Cognition, 8, 215–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2013). The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 107, 8993–8999.

  • Prevost, M., Carrier, M. E., Chowne, G., Zelkowitz, P., Joseph, L., & Gold, I. (2014). The Reading the Mind in the Eyes test: Validation of a French version and exploration of cultural variations in a multi-ethnic city. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 19, 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, E., Wood, L. A., & Whiten, A. (2017). Adaptive cultural transmission biases in children and nonhuman primates. Infant Behavior and Development, 48, 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reindl, E., & Tennie, C. (2018). Young children fail to generate an additive effect in an open-ended construction task. PLoS One, 13, e0197828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: A critical review of neuroimaging data. Neuroscience & BioBehavioral Reviews, 64, 421–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynaud, E., Navarro, J., Lesourd, M., & Osiurak, F. (2019). To watch is to work: A critical review of neuroimaging data on Tool-use Observation Network (ToON). Neuropsychology Review, 29, 484–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. C., Besche, C., & Widlöcher, D. (1997). Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: A non-verbal exploration with comic strips. Schizophrenia Research, 25, 199–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 186–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, D., & Hecht, E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 114, 7861–7868.

  • Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 17, 1473–1787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasielewski, H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25, 161–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27, 34–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & Erdal, D. (2012). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2119–2129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilks, M., Redshaw, J., Mushin, I., & Nielsen, M. (2019). A cross-cultural investigation of children’s willingness to imitate prosocial and antisocial groups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 185, 164–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. (2013). Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning. Developmental Review, 33, 341–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, L. A., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. (2015). Does a peer model’s task proficiency influence children’s solution choice and innovation? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 190–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwirner, E., & Thornton, A. (2015). Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Scientific Reports, 5, 16781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Grégoire Desquiens for assistance in data collection and Célia Camus for assistance in data processing.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from ANR (Agence Nationale pour la Recherche; Project “Cognition and tool-use economy” ECOTOOL; ANR-14-CE30-0015-01), and was performed within the framework of the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to François Osiurak.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(PDF 191 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Osiurak, F., Cretel, C., Duhau-Marmon, N. et al. The Pedagogue, the Engineer, and the Friend. Hum Nat 31, 462–482 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09379-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09379-0

Keywords

Navigation