Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Phenomenal consciousness, access consciousness and self across waking and dreaming: bridging phenomenology and neuroscience

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The distinction between phenomenal and access consciousness is central to debates about consciousness and its neural correlates. However, this distinction has often been limited to the domain of perceptual (visual) experiences. On the basis of dream phenomenology and neuroscientific findings this paper suggests a theoretical framework which extends this distinction to dreaming, also in terms of plausible neural correlates. In this framework, phenomenal consciousness is involved in both waking perception and dreaming, whereas access consciousness is weakened, but not fully eliminated, during dreaming. However, access consciousness is more active during lucid dreaming. The proposed framework accounts for different aspects of dream phenomenology, including levels of integration of perceptual, cognitive and affective features in dreams, bizarreness, dream amnesia and the occurrence of meta-awareness and accessibility in lucid dreaming. Self-related experiences and their neural substrates are suggested to be differently involved in waking cognition and dreaming. Further, phenomenal consciousness during both waking and dream experiences involve widespread recurrent interactions and convergence-divergence zones in the thalamo-cortico-limbic system, activated before conscious access in global workspace areas. Finally, we discuss the relationships of the proposed framework with other neurocognitive theories and models of consciousness and major theories of dreaming, and propose novel experimental predictions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1998). Metaphors of consciousness and attention in the brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 58–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 47–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J., Ramsoy, T. Z., & Laureys, S. (2003). Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 671–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J., Franklin, S., & Ramsoy, T. Z. (2013). Global workspace dynamics: cortical “binding and propagation” enables conscious contents. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Battelli, L., Pascual-Leone, A., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). The ‘when’ pathway of the right parietal lobe. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 204–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi, E., & Brooks, D. C. (1963). Pontine reticular formation: relation to lateral geniculate nucleus during deep sleep. Science, 171, 270–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 46–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioural and Brains Sciences, 30, 481–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2011). Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 567–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bókkon, I., Dai, J., & Antal, I. (2010). Picture representation during REM dreams: a redox molecular hypothesis. Biosystems, 100, 79–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, A. R., et al. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2(15)O PET study. Brain, 120, 1173–1197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. (1911). The intrinsic factor in the act of progression in the mammal. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 84, 308–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. A., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 211–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience --Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1124, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, S., & Watson, R. (1985). Individual differences in memory for dreams: the role of cognitive skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 841–864.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calkins, M. W. (1893). A statistical study of pseudo-chromesthesia and of mental forms. American Journal of Psychology, 5, 439–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Cosmeli, D., Legrand, D., & Thompson, E. (2011). Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 104–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chua, E. F., & Ahmed, R. (2016). Electrical stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex improves memory monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 85, 74–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicogna, P., & Bosinelli, M. (2001). Consciousness during dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 26–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colace, C., Claps, M., Antognoli, A., Sperandio, R., Sardi, D., & Benedetti, A. (2010). Limbic system activity and drug dreaming in drug-addicted subjects. Neuropsychoanalysis, 12, 201–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collerton, D., & Perry, E. (2011). Dreaming and hallucinations- continuity or discontinuity? Perspectives from dementia with Lewy bodies. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1016–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and the human awareness. Nature Review Neuroscience, 10, 59–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daprati, E., et al. (2000). Recognition of self produced movement in a case of severe neglect. Neurocase, 6, 477–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Gennaro, L., Marzano, C., Cipolli, C., & Ferrara, M. (2012). How we remember the stuff that dreams are made of: neurobiological approaches to the brain mechanisms of dream recall. Behavioral Brain Research, 226, 592–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2005). Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: a neuronal model for inattentional blindness. PLoS Biology, 3, e141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., & Changeux, J. P. (2003). A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 8520–8525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., & Changeux, J. P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States America, 95, 14529–14534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 204–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Sterpenich, V., & Schwartz, S. (2011). Cognitive and emotional processes during dreaming: a neuroimaging view. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 998–1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. (2011a). The neural substrate for dreaming: is it a subsystem of the default network? Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1163–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. (2011b). The neural substrate for dreaming: is it a subsystem of the default network? Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1163–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2003). The scientific study of dreams: Neural networks, cognitive development, and content analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E., Nelson, A. L., Leghari, M. A., et al. (2009). Neural systems mediating field and observer memories. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2239–2251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 313–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (2003). Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage, 18, 324–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, T., Venneri, A., Simone, A. M., et al. (2010). The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 81, 276–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fell, J., Klaver, P., Lehnertz, K., Grunwald, T., Schaller, C., Elger, C. E., & Fernandez, G. (2001). Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nature Neurosciences, 4, 1259–1264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fell, J., Fernandez, G., Lutz, M. T., Kockelmann, E., Burr, W., Schaller, C., Elger, C. E., & Helmstaedter, C. (2006). Rhinal-hippocampal connectivity determines memory formation during sleep. Brain, 129, 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in primate visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A., & Kurtzman, H. S. (1981). Mapping the visual field in mental imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychoolgy, 110, 501–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fosse, M. J., Fosse, R., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. J. (2003). Dreaming and episodic memory: a functional dissociation? Journal Cognition and Neuroscience., 15, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, D. (1985). Dreaming: A cognitive-psychological analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., & Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as mind wandering: evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams (Joyce Crick, Trans.). London: Oxford University Press.

  • Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalising. Philosophical Transactions, 358, 459–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., Clémenceau, S., Hasboun, D., et al. (2009). Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biology, 7(3), e1000061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 14–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghin, M. (2003). Book review: being no one: the self-model theory of subjectivity, by Thomas metzinger. Human Nature Review, 3, 450–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1996). The content analysis of dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 803–813.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., Stickgold, R., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (1998). The neuropsychology of REM sleep dreaming. NeuroReport, 9, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 793–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y. (2013). Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep. Science, 340, 639–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Edison, S. C. (2003). When autobiographical memory begins. Developmental Review, 23, 471–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, T. J. (1874). On the hypothesis that animals are automata, and its history. Nature, 10, 362–66. Science and Culture, and Other Essays; CE 1: 199–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides, A. A., et al. (2009). MEG identifies dorsal medial brain activations during sleep. NeuroImage, 44, 455–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal qualia. The Philosophical Quarterly, 32, 127–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1892). Psychology, briefer course. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., Kahan, T. L., & Raye, C. L. (1984). Dreams and reality monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 113, 329–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jouvet, M. (1962). Resercher sur les structures nerveuses et les mechanismes responsables des differentes phases du sommeil physiologique. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 100, 125–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Dell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, D., & Hobson, A. (2005). Theory of mind in dreaming: awareness of feelings and thoughts of others in dreams. Dreaming, 15, 48–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1995). Awareness of one’s own body: An attentional theory of its nature, development, and brain basis. In J. L. Bermudez, A. Marcel, & N. Eilan (Eds.), The body and the self (pp. 205–223). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. (Ed.). (1991). Neuronal cooperativity. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1990). Thalamic and cortical mechanisms of attention suggested by recent positron emission tomographic experiments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 358–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V. (2003). Why attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V. (2007). Sue Ned Block!: making a better case for P-consciousness. Behavioural and Brains Sciences, 30, 481–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 204–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 571–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legrand, D. (2003). How not to find the neural signature of self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 544–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Limosani, I., D’Agostino, A., Manzone, M. L., & Scarone, S. (2011). The dreaming brain/mind, consciousness and psychosis. Conscious Cogniton, 20, 987–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linas, R. (2013). The olivo-cerebellar system: a key to understanding the functional significance of intrinsic oscillatory brain properties. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 7, 96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Llinas, R., & Ribary, U. (1994). Coherent 40-Hz oscillation characterizes dream state in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, 2078–2081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Llinas, R., Ribary, U., Contreras, U., & Pedroarena, C. (1998). The neural basis for consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 353, 1841–1849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Man, K., Kaplan, J. T., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (2014). Neural convergence and divergence in the mammalian cerebral cortex: from experimental neuroanatomy to functional neuroimaging. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 521, 4097–4111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maquet, P., et al. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature, 383, 163–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maquet, P., Ruby, P., Maudoux, A., et al. (2005). Human cognition during REM sleep and the activity profile within the frontal and parietal cortices: a reappraisal of functional neuroimaging data. In Progress in Brain Research.(pp 219–227). Laureys Elsevier.

  • Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macre, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCormick, L., Nielsen, T., Ptito, M., et al. (1997). REM sleep mentation in right hemispherectomized patients. Neuropsicologia, 35, 695–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, H. G. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2004). Being no one. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2013). Why are dreams interesting for philosophers? The example of minimal selfhood plus an agenda for future research. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 746.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, J., & Damasio, A. R. (2009). Convergence and divergence in neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 32, 376–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murre, J. M. (1996). TraceLink: a model of amnesia and consolidation of memory. Hippocampus, 6, 675–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 475–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83, 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2009). Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 88–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., et al. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain- A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 450–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Occhionero, M., & Cicogna, P. C. (2011). Autoscopic phenomena and one’s own body representation in dreams. Consciousness & Cognition, 20, 1009–1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Occhionero, M., Cicogna, P., Natale, V., Esposito, M. J., & Bosinelli, M. (2005). Representation of self in SWS and REM dreams. Sleep and Hypnosis, 7, 77–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perogamvros, L., Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., & Schwartz, S. (2013). Sleep and dreaming are for important matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pia, L., Neppi-Modona, M., Ricci, R., & Berti, A. (2004). The anatomy of anosognosia for hemiplegia: a meta-analysis. Cortex, 40, 367–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Ballantin.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., Tagini, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). Mindfulness and the cognitive neuroscience of attention and awareness. Zygon, 45, 627–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). An adaptive workspace hypothesis about the neural correlates of consciousness: insights from neuroscience and meditation studies. In N. Srinivasan (Ed.) Progress in brain research: Attention (pp. 161–180), vol. 176, Amsterdam: Elsevier. pp 171–180.

  • Raffone, A., & van Leeuwen, C. (2001). Activation and coherence in memory processes: revisiting the PDP approach to retrieval. Connection Science, 13, 349–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., & Wolters, G. (2001). A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 766–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., & van Leeuwen, C. (2014). The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1641), 20130215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E., & Sneyder, A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage, 37, 1083–1090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., et al. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 676–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A. (2006). Inner presence: Consciousness as a biological phenomenon. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A., & Tarkko, K. (2002). Binding in dreams. Journal for Consciousness Study, 9, 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, M. G. (2013). What I make up when I wake up: anti-experience views and narrative fabrication of dreams. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, M. G., & Sutton, J. (2013). Self representation and perspectives in dreams. Philosophy Compass, 8, 1041–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2001). Effect of the subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 546–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel. A neuroimaging study of perspective taking with social emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 988–999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. (2003). Are life episodes replayed during dreaming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 325–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S., & Maquet, P. (2002). Sleep imaging and the neuropsychological assessment of dreams. Trends and Cognitive Sciences, 6, 23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, S., & Maquet, P. (2002). Sleep imaging and neuropsychological assessment of dreams. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastián, M. A. (2014). Dreams: an empirical Way to settle the discussion between cognitive and non-cognitive theories of consciousness. Synthese, 191, 263–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seeley, W. W., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2349–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherrington, C., (1906) The integrative action of nervous system. Yale University Press.

  • Siclari, F., Larocque, J. J., Postle, B. R., & Tononi, G. (2013). Assessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sikka, P., Valli, K., Virta, T., & Revonsuo, A. (2014). I know how you felt last night, or do I? Self- and external ratings of emotions in REM sleep dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 51–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simione, L., Raffone, A., Wolters, G., Salmas, P., Nakatani, C., Belardinelli, M., & van Leeuwen, C. (2012). VISA: a neurodynamic model for visuo-spatial working memory, attentional bilnk and conscious access. Psychological Review, 119, 739–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, J. S., & Spiers, H. J. (2003). Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 637–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, W. (2011). Dynamic formation of functional networks by synchronization. Neuron, 69, 191–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solms M., (1997). The neuropsychology of dreaming: A clinico-anatomical study. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Solms, M. (2000). Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Science, 23, 843–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, G. L., & Graham, G. (2000). When self-consciousness breaks: Alien voices and inserted thoughts. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauch, I. Meier, B., (1996). In search of dreams: Results of experimental dream research, State. University of New York Press.

  • Tagini, A., & Raffone, A. (2010). The ‘I’ and the ‘Me’ in self-referential awareness: a neurocognitive hypothesis. Cognitive Processing, 11, 9–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., Sporns, O., & Edelman, G. M. (1992). Reentry and the problem of integrating multiple brain areas: simulation of dynamic integration in the visual system. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 310–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valli, K., & Revonsuo, A. (2009). The threat simulation theory in light of recent empirical evidence: a review. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 17–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1995). Resonant cell assemblies: a new approach to cognitive functions and neuronal synchrony. Biological Reserch, 28, 81–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Lachaux, J., Rodriguez, E., & Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 229–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Hobson, A., Paulus, W., Koppehele-Gossel, J., Klimke, A., & Nitsche, M. A. (2014). Induction of self awareness in dreams through frontal low current stimulation of gamma activity. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 810–812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, U., Hobson, A., et al. (2015). What is the State-of-the-Art on lucid dreaming? - Recent advances and questions for future research. In Open MIND: 38 (T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 553–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windt, J. M. (2010). The immersive spatiotemporal hallucination model of dreaming. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9, 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windt, J. M., & Metzinger, T. (2007). The philosophy of dreaming and self-consciousness: What happens to the experiential subject during the dream state? In The new science of dreaming: Volume 3. Cultural and theoretical perspectives. Praeger perspectives (pp. 193–247). Westport: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolters, G., & Raffone, A. (2008). Coherence and recurrency: maintenance, control and integration in working memory. Cognitive Processing, 9, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers of “Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences” for their very useful remarks and suggestions leading to a remarkably improved manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antonino Raffone.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pantani, M., Tagini, A. & Raffone, A. Phenomenal consciousness, access consciousness and self across waking and dreaming: bridging phenomenology and neuroscience. Phenom Cogn Sci 17, 175–197 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-016-9491-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-016-9491-x

Keywords

Navigation