Introduction

The success of multinational home products company IKEA in selling sustainable products ($1.13 billion in 2014, an increase of 58% over 2013) clearly signifies positive consumer responses to a retailer’s sustainability efforts (EcoWatch.com2015). According to the literature, such efforts may lead to increases in product preference (Luchs et al. 2010), firm market value (Luo and Bhattacharya 2006), positive company evaluations and purchase intentions (Mohr and Webb 2005; Sen and Bhattacharya 2001; Wagner et al. 2009; Yoon et al. 2006), repeat patronage and recommendation intentions (Vlachos et al. 2009), brand choice (Barone et al. 2000; Henderson and Arora 2010), customer donations to corporate-supported nonprofit groups (Lichtenstein et al. 2004), and other forms of consumer support (Handelman and Arnold 1999).

On the other hand, many consumers are skeptical (e.g., Hsu 2011), and recent research (Luchs et al. 2010; Wagner et al. 2009; Yoon et al. 2006) suggests that positive consumer responses are not guaranteed. Some consumers are more likely to respond positively than others, and some circumstances strengthen or weaken the effects. From a marketing perspective, knowledge on such moderators of the sustainability efforts–consumer responses relationship is needed to help retailers in making their efforts more effective.Footnote 1 In addition, understanding how (through which mediating mechanisms) these efforts translate into positive consumer responses would provide further assistance. Despite growing research on the outcomes of sustainability, extant research into the underlying mechanisms is scarce (Bhattacharya et al. 2009; Yoon et al. 2006). This study therefore aims to address how (through which mechanisms) and when (under which conditions) retailers’ sustainability efforts translate into positive consumer responses. We thereby focus on personal versus social factors, inspired by previous studies in the field of marketing and consumer behavior, in particular pro-environmental behavior.

Personal and social (or subjective) norms have a prominent role in the pro-environmental behavior literature, affecting, for instance, consumer recycling behavior (Oom Do Valle et al. 2005), and drivers’ intentions to reduce their car use for commuting (Wall et al. 2007). When consumers have stronger personal and social norms favoring certain behavior, they should be motivated to conform to these norms and behave accordingly (Schuler and Cording 2006). It is particularly interesting to jointly examine personal and social norms since it sheds light on whether sustainable consumer behavior is motivated by norms that are internally held or externally imposed, or both. Some studies in the pro-environmental behavior field show that the effect of social norms becomes nonsignificant if personal norms are included (e.g., Wall et al., 2007), or the other way around (e.g., Bamberg and Schmidt 2003). In the sustainable or ethical marketing literature, the joint examination of personal and social norms has received less attention, although work by Shaw and colleagues examined the effects of subjective norms and a construct similar to personal norms, ethical obligation, on intention to purchase fair trade grocery products. Their studies show mixed effects as well (Ozcaglar-Toulouse et al. 2006; Shaw and Shiu 2002, 2003; Shaw et al. 2000). In further support of our research on the effects of personal and social norms on sustainable consumer behavior are the suggestions by Vitell (2015), who argues that future research should focus on identifying the characteristics of people who are socially responsible and of those who are not, as well as the influence of family and friends on ethical decision making.

To understand how (through which mechanisms) retailers’ sustainability efforts translate into positive consumer responses, we build upon work by Sen and Bhattacharya (2001) and Handelman and Arnold (1999). Again, we are triggered by personal versus social factors. The few studies that have investigated the mediating mechanisms underlying the sustainability efforts–consumer responses relationship mainly focused on personal factors, in particular emotion-laden processes such as consumer–firm emotional attachment (Vlachos 2012) and consumer–company congruence or identification (Sen and Bhattacharya 2001). An alternative route, proposed by Handelman and Arnold (1999), focused on a social factor, namely organizational legitimacy, which is “a generalized perception or assumption that the actions of an entity are desirable, proper, or appropriate within some socially constructed system of norms, values, beliefs, and definitions” (Suchman 1995, p. 574). Given that many companies try to improve their legitimacy and secure their license to operate through sustainability efforts (Aguilera et al. 2007), it is surprising that this route has not been examined in more detail in the marketing literature. Our study aims to advance theory by examining these personal (consumer–company identification) and social routes (perceived organizational legitimacy) simultaneously, allowing for a theoretical and empirical comparison of the two routes and providing insight into the workings and relative importance of each of them.

In the remainder of this article, we begin with a theoretical background and development of hypotheses, followed by a presentation of the scenario-based experimental design we used to test the hypotheses. Then we report the results and conclude with theoretical and managerial implications, limitations, and suggestions for further research.

Theoretical Background and Hypotheses Development

Sustainability, Corporate Social Responsibility, and Consumer Social Responsibility

Sustainability has become an important theme in society. Governments, companies, and individuals increasingly express their concern for the natural and social environment, which they are part of and which is affected by their behavior. In 1987, the World Commission on Environment and Development published “Our Common Future” in which they called for sustainable development, or development that “meets the needs of the present without compromising the ability of future generations to meet their own needs” (p. 43). Accordingly, companies have adopted the idea of the triple bottom line, which holds that business performance should be assessed on social justice, environmental quality, and economic prosperity also referred to as People, Planet, Profit (Elkington 1999).

Related to sustainability is the concept of Corporate Social Responsibility (CSR), whereby a CSR-focused business can be defined as one that “proactively offers social benefits or public service, and voluntarily minimizes practices that harm society, regardless of any legal requirements” (Vitell 2015, p. 767). For CSR to be successful however, the assistance of consumers is needed: They should be willing to support companies that practice CSR and buy their products or services. Without consumer support, CSR would often not lead to business success.

This consumer support for CSR is called Consumer Social Responsibility (CnSR) by Vitell (2015). More precisely, consumer ethics/social responsibility is defined as “the moral principles and standards that guide the behaviors of individuals as they obtain, use, and dispose of goods and services” (Muncy and Vitell 1992, p. 298). Based on this definition, two consumer responsibilities are distinguished. First, consumers should act ethically toward other stakeholders when they obtain, use, and dispose of goods and services (i.e., consumer ethics). Second, consumers have a responsibility to society as a whole to avoid practices that harm society and to act proactively for social benefit with regard to obtaining, using, and disposing of products (i.e., CnSR) (Vitell 2015). The latter is in line with the fifth dimension of the consumer ethics scale, which constitutes positive (ethical) actions of consumers, namely “doing good/recycling.” This dimension has been recently added to the four original dimensions of the consumer ethics scale (actively benefiting from illegal activities, passively benefiting, actively benefiting from questionable (but perceived to be legal) acts, and no harm/no foul), thereby increasing the scope of consumer ethics to include CnSR (Muncy and Vitell 1992; Vitell 2015; Vitell and Muncy 2005).

Retailers’ Sustainability Efforts and Consumer Responses

We examine consumer social responsibility in relation to obtaining products from retailers by studying the effects of two sustainability efforts covering two elements of the marketing mix: assortment sustainability (Product) and distribution sustainability (Distribution). Building an attractive assortment and getting products close to consumers are two important functions of retailers through which they serve their customers. These functions offer considerable opportunities to increase retailers’ sustainability, for example by including environmentally friendly or fair trade products in the assortment. We focus on environmental aspects of sustainability and define assortment sustainability as the environmental friendliness of products sold by the retail store, according to their overall life cycle (from raw materials to waste). In terms of distribution, we examine how stores located in urban areas obtain supplies (i.e., urban distribution), which directly affects the quality of life of consumers, citizens, and communities in the form of local emissions, noise, and congestion (Quak and De Koster 2007). In line with efforts to optimize urban distribution (Browne et al. 2005), we define distribution sustainability as the environmental friendliness of the method in which a store is supplied. The examination of assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability answers the call for research to consider multiple sustainability efforts and their unique effects (Aguilera et al. 2007; Godfrey et al. 2009; Peloza and Shang 2011). A systematic review by Peloza and Shang (2011) revealed that most researchers use a single effort.

In general, companies’ sustainability efforts prompt positive consumer responses (Brown and Dacin 1997; Handelman and Arnold 1999; Mohr and Webb 2005), because most people regard sustainability positively and associate it with moral fairness and universal justice norms (Aguilera et al. 2007). Since these positive effects can be very much expected, we do not state formal hypotheses about them. Following the definition of CnSR (Vitell 2015), socially responsible consumers have the responsibility to society as a whole to shop at stores with a sustainable assortment and distribution. However, not all consumers act in a socially responsible way when obtaining products. For them, other (self-interest related) attributes such as price might be more important. The difference between socially responsible and nonresponsible consumers might be caused by certain personal characteristics or social conditions (Vitell 2015). Therefore, we focus on personal and social factors that are expected to mediate and moderate the sustainability efforts–consumer responses relationship. In doing that, we consider four responses that are relevant to retailers: store evaluations (Yoon et al. 2006), intentions to shop at the store (Handelman and Arnold 1999), word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more (Zhang and Bloemer 2008).

Mediation by Identification and Store Legitimacy

We propose two theoretical concepts that offer a personal and social route through which retailers’ sustainability efforts may translate into positive consumer responses: consumers’ identification with the retail store and perceived store legitimacy, respectively. Identification is “the perception of oneness with or belongingness to” an organization (Ashforth and Mael 1989, p. 21). According to social identity theory, people often identify with organizations (or subgroups in organizations) to which they belong (Bhattacharya et al. 1995; Mael and Ashforth 1992). Such organizational identification helps people achieve their desire for self-definition (Ashforth and Mael 1989; Bhattacharya and Sen 2003). They do not need to be formal members of an organization (e.g., employees) to identify with it; consumers can identify with organizations from which they buy products (Lichtenstein et al. 2010; Sen and Bhattacharya 2001). Since sustainability is generally regarded positively (Aguilera et al. 2007), we propose that consumers are more likely to identify with stores that employ sustainability efforts and “do good,” because such identification can enhance their self-esteem (Bergami and Bagozzi 2000; Mael and Ashforth 1992). Because organizational identification in turn enhances people’s support for an organization (Sen and Bhattacharya 2001), we posit:

H1:

The effects of assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability on (a) store evaluations, (b) shopping intentions, (c) word-of-mouth intentions, and (d) willingness to pay more are mediated by consumers’ identification with the store.

The social route-mediator we propose is organizational legitimacy, which reflects the desirability or appropriateness of an organization’s actions within institutional norms (Suchman 1995). Legitimacy is a key construct in institutional theory, such that

Organizations are driven to incorporate the practices and procedures defined by prevailing rationalized concepts of organizational work and institutionalized in society. Organizations that do so increase their legitimacy and their survival prospects, independent of the immediate efficacy of the acquired practices and procedures (Meyer and Rowan 1977, p. 340).

Legitimacy is necessary to obtain stakeholder (e.g., consumer) support (Kumar and Das 2007; Zimmerman and Zeitz 2002) and can reflect pragmatic (i.e., self-interested) or moral (i.e., normative) considerations (Suchman 1995). Because social responsibility has become an important institutional norm in societies nowadays (Aguilera et al. 2007), we argue that a store’s sustainability efforts contribute to consumers’ perceptions of the store’s legitimacy, particularly its moral legitimacy, because sustainability goes beyond consumers’ self-interest. In turn, store legitimacy prompts positive consumer responses (Handelman and Arnold 1999). Therefore, we hypothesize:

H2:

The effects of assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability on (a) store evaluations, (b) shopping intentions, (c) word-of-mouth intentions, and (d) willingness to pay more are mediated by store legitimacy.

Moderating Effects of Personal and Social Norms

Not all consumers react to sustainability efforts similarly (Aguilera et al. 2007). Sen and Bhattacharya (2001) find, for example, that consumers who support a specific CSR domain react more positively to a company’s CSR actions in that same domain. Building on two influential behavioral theories from psychology that have a prominent role in the pro-environmental behavior literature (norm-activation theory, Schwartz 1968; theory of planned behavior, Ajzen 1991), we propose that the effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses depend on personal and social norms. Personal norms reflect feelings of moral obligation to perform specific behaviors to benefit others, according to an internalized structure of values and without regard for social or material benefits to the self (Schwartz 1977; Schwartz and Howard1980). Social norms reflect a person’s perceptions of what important others think of performing a specific behavior, or “the perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform the behavior” (Ajzen 1991, p. 188). We propose that the relationship between sustainability efforts and consumer responses is strengthened by both personal and social norms favoring shopping at environmentally friendly stores, for several reasons. First, people are strongly motivated to behave consistently with their self-expectations and personal norms, because doing so results in feelings of pride, enhanced self-esteem, and other positive self-evaluations. Violations of personal norms instead spark negative self-evaluations, such as guilt or self-deprecation (Schwartz 1977). Thus, people who feel a stronger moral obligation to shop at environmentally friendly stores likely respond more positively to retail stores’ sustainability efforts. Second, people have a fundamental desire to affiliate with others and behave accurately, both in order to maintain a positive self-concept. Through conforming to social norms, people seek to achieve these goals (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004). Therefore, people who perceive more social pressure to shop at environmentally friendly stores likely respond more positively to retail stores’ sustainability efforts.

H3:

Personal norms strengthen the effects of assortment sustainability on (a) consumers’ identification with the store and (b) store legitimacy.

H4:

Personal norms strengthen the effects of distribution sustainability on (a) consumers’ identification with the store and (b) store legitimacy.

H5:

Social norms strengthen the effects of assortment sustainability on (a) consumers’ identification with the store and (b) store legitimacy.

H6:

Social norms strengthen the effects of distribution sustainability on (a) consumers’ identification with the store and (b) store legitimacy.

We depict our proposed conceptual model in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model

Method

Design

To test the hypotheses, we used a 2 (low/high assortment sustainability) × 2 (low/high distribution sustainability) × 2 (store type: office supplies/personal care) between-subjects design. Eight scenarios covered the different combinations of the three factors. Each scenario described a fictitious store. By including two store types, we aimed to increase the generalizability of our findings. Office supplies and personal care products clearly differ (e.g., in terms of use), but both are available in environmentally friendly versions in reality, and hence, our scenarios remained credible in both cases.

Stimuli

In the scenarios, we used real-life sustainability initiatives from the Netherlands to manipulate assortment and distribution sustainability. For assortment sustainability, we manipulated whether the store’s products had an environmental quality mark (Milieukeur2011). Products certified with the Milieukeur mark are demonstrably environmentally friendlier in their overall life cycle (from raw materials to waste) than products without the mark. For the manipulation of distribution sustainability, we described the store’s participation or nonparticipation in a sustainable urban distribution initiative (Binnenstadservice). This initiative aims to reduce the amount of freight traffic in city centers in order to improve air quality, livability, and accessibility of city centers. Information about the Milieukeur mark, the Binnenstadservice initiative, and their environmental consequences appeared in the form of a newspaper article (“More stores going green”; see “Appendix 1”).

Half of the scenarios depicted a fictitious store selling office supplies (OfficeSupplies), and the other half portrayed a fictitious store selling personal care products (Hair&Care), for the store-type manipulation. Both store descriptions mentioned wide assortments, high-quality products, and affordable prices. The manipulations were checked with two manipulation check items asking: How does OfficeSupplies (Hair&Care) score on the following two points: Environmental friendliness of the products in the assortment [very bad–very good] and environmental friendliness of the method in which they are supplied [very bad–very good].

Dependent Variables, Moderators, and Mediators

This study featured four ultimate dependent variables (store evaluation, shopping intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more), two mediators (identification and legitimacy), and two moderators (personal norms and social norms). To measure these constructs, we used existing scales and adapted them as necessary to fit our context. We used a double back-translation procedure to translate the items from English to Dutch.

Ultimate Dependent Variables

We used three items to measure store evaluations, adapted from Scholder Ellen et al. (2000) (first item) and Mohr and Webb (2005) (second and third items). The three items measuring shopping intentions were also based on Mohr and Webb (2005). For word-of-mouth intentions, we used three items from Zhang and Bloemer (2008). Finally, we employed two items from Zhang and Bloemer (2008) and a third (reversed) item from Bloemer and Odekerken-Schröder (2007) to measure willingness to pay more.

Mediators

For the measure of consumers’ identification with the store, we used Bergami and Bagozzi’s (2000) organizational identification scale, which consists of an eight-point visual scale that assesses the degree of overlap between one’s own and the organization’s identity, as well as a second (nonvisual) item measured on a seven-point Likert scale. The pilot test results (coefficient α = .67, r = .50) prompted the addition of a third item that we formulated as: “OfficeSupplies’ (Hair&Care’s) values match my own values.” In this item, we used the key component of overlap (‘match’) of the original two items and combined it with values, which are an important element of one’s own and an organization’s identity. Handelman and Arnold (1999) developed eight items to measure social as well as pragmatic legitimacy. We used the four social legitimacy items to measure store legitimacy, because we are interested in the moral aspects of legitimacy rather than the self-interest aspects (Suchman 1995) since sustainability goes beyond consumers’ self-interest.

Moderators

The three items for personal norms came from Bosnjak et al. (2005). To measure social norms, we used one item from Ajzen and Madden (1986), a second item adapted from Smith et al. (2008), and a third item based on Harland et al. (1999). All items appear in Table 1, which also includes the control variable of scenario credibility (measured with two items; Sen and Bhattacharya 2001).

Table 1 Construct items

Pilot Experiment

We pretested our scenarios and measurement instrument among 16 consumers, who suggested wording adjustments, though they judged the scenarios as credible overall. Next, we conducted a pilot test among 130 undergraduate and graduate students. The analyses of variance (ANOVAs) revealed that the manipulations worked: For the assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability manipulations, the respective manipulation check scores were significantly higher in the high conditions than in the low conditions. The ANOVAs also revealed a few small confounding effects; the distribution sustainability manipulation significantly influenced the assortment sustainability manipulation check, and the assortment sustainability × distribution sustainability interaction significantly influenced the distribution sustainability manipulation check. However, the effect sizes (ω2) were close to 0 (.00 or .01) and thereby much smaller than the effect sizes of the intended manipulations, which indicated no serious confounding problems (Perdue and Summers, 1986).

Most scales performed well in terms of measurement properties. Identification had a somewhat low coefficient alpha (.67) and item correlation (.50), so we included an extra identification item in the main experiment. A low factor loading on the second social norm item led us to reformulate it.

Procedure

The main experiment included members of an Internet panel of a market research company, 2500 of whom received an invitation to participate in the study. This group was representative of the Dutch adult population in terms of age, education, and income. A cover letter stated that the study was interested in consumers’ opinions about stores. People gave their informed consent before they continued. Panel members received 1 euro if they completed the study. A total of 672 people participated, resulting in a 27% response rate.

After respondents read a newspaper article and a description of the store (see “Appendix 1”), they provided their store evaluation, shopping intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more scores. They also indicated their degree of identification with the store and rated the store’s legitimacy. Then they completed the manipulation check items. In the next section, they answered the personal and social norms items and indicated how familiar they were with the Milieukeur mark and Binnenstadservice initiative. They also stated whether they had bought office supplies (personal care products) in the previous 6 months. In the third section, respondents provided demographic information, and in the fourth section, they rated the scenario’s credibility and believability.

Sample

The sample (N = 672) consisted of 43% men and 57% women. The age distribution was as follows: 18–25 years 5%, 26–35 years 12%, 36–45 years 15%, 46–55 years 22%, 56–65 years 31%, and older than 65 years 15%. Regarding education, 1% of the respondents finished primary education, 35% secondary education, 29% middle-level vocational education, 26% higher-level vocational education, and 9% university. Finally, 38% of the respondents reported a net household income of less than 2000 euro per month, 35% had incomes between 2000 and 3000 euro per month, and 27% reported an income of more than 3000 euro per month.

We tested for nonresponse bias using the procedure recommended by Armstrong and Overton (1977) and found no statistically significant differences in construct means for early and late respondents, indicating that the study did not suffer from a serious nonresponse bias.

Measurement Properties

To assess the validity and reliability of our measurement scales, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis in AMOS 23. Despite a significant χ2 statistic (χ2(324) = 997.44, p < .01), the fit indices indicated a good model fit: confirmatory fit index = .97, Tucker–Lewis index = .96, incremental fit index = .97, and root-mean-squared error of approximation = .06. All but one of the standardized factor loadings were significant and above the recommended value of .6 (Bagozzi and Yi 1988), suggesting convergent validity (Anderson and Gerbing 1988). The factor loading of the third willingness to pay item was only .32; therefore, we deleted it. The composite reliability and coefficient alpha scores were well above the recommended values of .6 (Bagozzi and Yi 1988) and .7 (Nunnally 1978), respectively, and the average variance extracted (AVE) of each construct was greater than .5, as recommended by Fornell and Larcker (1981), in support of internal validity. We assessed discriminant validity by comparing the square root of the AVE of each construct to its correlations with the other latent constructs. The correlation between identification and store legitimacy was higher (.88) than the square root of the AVE of identification (.84).

An inspection of the items of both constructs in terms of their content prompted us to conclude that two of the four legitimacy items could be interpreted as measuring something other than perceptions of appropriateness within some socially constructed system of norms, because they could refer to perceptions of appropriateness according to each consumer’s individual norms: “OfficeSupplies sets an example for how (other) stores selling office supplies should behave/conduct their activities.” If someone agrees with these items, OfficeSupplies operates in line with what one finds important and thus one will likely identify with OfficeSupplies, especially if moral aspects such as sustainability are concerned. To increase the discriminant validity of both constructs, we deleted the two legitimacy items, which resulted in a square root of the AVE of identification (.84) above the correlation between identification and store legitimacy (.80), in support of discriminant validity (Fornell and Larcker 1981). For all other pairs of constructs, this criterion was also met. We provide the construct items and measurement properties in Table 1, as well as the descriptive statistics, correlations, and square roots of AVEs in Table 2.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations

Common Method Variance

The measures for the theoretical constructs are all self-reported measures, which are deemed appropriate because we are interested in how consumers respond to retailers’ sustainability efforts in terms of their perceptions (identification, legitimacy), attitudes (store evaluations), intentions (shopping intentions and word-of-mouth intentions), and willingness to pay more. Furthermore, we are interested in how consumer responses to sustainability efforts are affected by their personal norms and perceived social norms. Self-reported measures may raise concerns about common method bias. We tried to reduce method bias by communicating to respondents that their answers were anonymous and that there were no right or wrong answers. According to Podsakoff et al. (2003, p. 888), “These procedures should reduce people’s evaluation apprehension and make them less likely to edit their responses to be more socially desirable, lenient, acquiescent, and consistent with how they think the researcher wants them to respond.” Furthermore, we pretested our questionnaire to avoid problems with ambiguous or complex items which may cause method biases (Podsakoff et al. 2003). Post hoc, we demonstrated construct validity and lack of overlap in items for different constructs as suggested by Conway and Lance (2010).

Podsakoff et al. (2003) recommend to implement statistical remedies to control for method biases, for example the single-common-method-factor approach, whereby all items load onto an unmeasured common method factor. Then, the hypothesized relationships are analyzed to see whether they remain significant. Although frequently used, Conway and Lance (2010, p. 331) see this approach as “logically indefensible as it may easily remove trait variance when multiple traits have a common cause.” Richardson et al. (2009) found that this approach actually decreased the accuracy of estimated correlations compared to if no correction was applied, in cases where method variance was present and true correlations were greater than zero.

Lance et al. (2010) show that correlations between constructs measured by the same method are simultaneously attenuated due to measurement error and inflated due to common method variance. Thus, in order to obtain accurate estimates of correlations, they should be corrected for both these attenuating and inflationary effects. We adopted this approach and found that all theoretically interesting monomethod correlations (i.e., correlations in line with hypothesized effects between constructs measured by the same method) remained significant (p < .05) (see “Appendix 2”), thereby providing confidence in the results we found for our structural model.

Data Preparation and Hypotheses Testing Approach

We effects-coded the categorical variables and mean-centered the continuous moderating variables before creating the interaction terms. Mean-centering served to reduce multicollinearity between the interaction terms and their composite variables; it also supported the interpretation of first-order effects of variables that were also part of interaction terms. When interactions are included (e.g., assortment sustainability × personal norms), first-order effects no longer represent main effects, but instead become conditional effects—that is, the effect of a predictor or moderator variable (e.g., assortment sustainability) on a dependent variable (e.g., store evaluation) when the other variable (personal norms) is 0 (which is equal to its mean if the variable has been mean-centered) (Aiken and West 1991).

Before we started the hypothesis testing, we determined whether the data from the two store-type contexts could be pooled, and thus, whether the measurement and structural model did not differ between contexts. Multiple-group analysis in AMOS demonstrated that the measurement weights, structural weights, structural covariances, and structural residuals were invariant across the store-type contexts, which means that we could pool the data. Then to test our hypotheses, we used structural equation modeling (SEM) in AMOS. Although SEM has been used occasionally in experimental studies, it offers important advantages over ANOVA and regression analysis (Bagozzi and Yi 1988). First, the measurement models can be represented fully by latent constructs involving multiple measurement items. Second, SEM can explicitly model measurement error, which results in greater statistical power. Third, the relationships between multiple independent and dependent constructs can be estimated simultaneously, resulting in more accurate estimates. Fourth, SEM offers not only better performance but also more convenience for mediation analyses (Iacobucci et al. 2007).

Results

Manipulation Checks and the Control Variable

The ANOVAs revealed a successful assortment sustainability manipulation (F(1, 670) = 353.01, p < .01). The means for assortment sustainability were significantly higher in the high assortment sustainability (M = 5.33) than in the low assortment sustainability (M = 3.20) conditions. The distribution sustainability manipulation was also successful (F(1, 670) = 439.16, p < .01; Mhigh = 5.58, Mlow = 3.06). Similar to the pilot test, we found a few, small confounding effects: The distribution sustainability manipulation significantly influenced the assortment sustainability manipulation check, and the assortment sustainability manipulation significantly affected the distribution sustainability manipulation check. However, the effect sizes (ω2) were .00 and thereby much smaller than the effect sizes of the intended manipulations, so confounding was not a serious problem (Perdue and Summers 1986).

Overall, the participants perceived the scenarios as rather credible (M = 4.42); they regarded the high assortment sustainability scenarios (M = 4.64) as significantly more credible than the low assortment sustainability scenarios (M = 4.19; F(1, 670) = 18.74, p < .01). Likewise, the high distribution sustainability scenarios (M = 4.61) appeared more credible than the low distribution sustainability scenarios (M = 4.21; F(1, 670) = 14.82, p < .01). To control for the potential confounding effects of scenario credibility with sustainability manipulations, we included credibility as a covariate in our subsequent analyses (cf. Sen and Bhattacharya 2001).

Model Overview

In Table 3, we report the results for a model in which we included the two manipulations and their interaction (Model 1) (resembling an ANOVA). In Table 4, we added the first-order and interaction effects of personal and social norms (Model 2). Both models contained the hypothesized indirect paths, as well as the corresponding direct paths, as required for mediation analyses (Iacobucci et al. 2007). We used bootstrapping procedures (5000 samples) to test for the significance of indirect and total effects (Zhao et al. 2010). A nested model comparison (to compare Model 1, the constrained model, with Model 2, the full model) showed that Model 2 fit the data significantly better than Model 1.

Table 3 Model 1: effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses
Table 4 Model 2: effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses including interactions

Manipulation Effects on Consumer Responses

As expected, the results in Tables 3 and 4 show that the total effects (direct + indirect effects) of the manipulations on the consumer response variables were positive and significant (p < .01). Remarkably, assortment sustainability (b = −.17, p < .01) and distribution sustainability (b = −.10, p < .05) had significant negative direct effects on shopping intentions when we controlled for indirect effects through identification and legitimacy. Distribution sustainability exerted a positive direct effect on store evaluations (b = .16, p < .01).

Mediation by identification and store legitimacy

To test for mediation, we followed Zhao et al. (2010), who recommend only one test (cf. Baron and Kenny’s [1986] three-step procedure): a bootstrap test of the indirect effects. This test revealed significant indirect effects (p < .01) of assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability on consumers’ store evaluations, shopping intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more. For these effects, mediation thus occurred by identification and store legitimacy together. To determine whether each of the mediators also mediated the relationships, we turned to the Sobel z test, because the bootstrapping procedure in AMOS does not provide confidence intervals for separate indirect effects through multiple mediators (Iacobucci et al. 2007).

We predicted in H1 that the effects of assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability on consumer responses would be mediated by identification. H1 received confirmation from the Sobel z test results (all z values > 2.58, which indicated that the indirect effects were significant at p < .01). H2, which predicted that the effects of both sustainability efforts would be mediated by store legitimacy, was also supported by the Sobel z test (all z values > 2.58). The estimates of the indirect effects of both sustainability efforts on consumer responses showed stronger effects through identification than through store legitimacy.

Using nested model comparisons, we compared the full model (containing both mediators) with two constrained models (in which the paths to and from one of the mediators equaled 0). The full model (χ2(609) = 1915.57, p < .01) fit the data significantly better than either a store legitimacy-only (Δχ2(14) = 600.86, p < .01) or identification-only (Δχ2(15) = 376.94, p < .01) model, which indicated that the two routes are indeed complementary. These findings also showed that the identification-only model performed better than the legitimacy-only model, which underlined the finding that the route via identification is the stronger route from sustainability efforts to consumer responses.

Moderating Effects of Personal and Social Norms

In H3 and H4, we stated that personal norms would strengthen the effects of assortment and distribution sustainability on consumers’ identification with the store and store legitimacy. We found support for the positive interaction effect between personal norms and assortment sustainability on identification (b = .21, p < .01) and legitimacy (b = .33, p < .01) (H3). Also, the effect of distribution sustainability on identification was strengthened by personal norms (b = .10, p < .01; H4a), but not the effect on legitimacy (b = .12, p > .05; H4b) (see Fig. 2, Panels A and B).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Illustration of interaction effects. a Interaction effect of assortment sustainability and personal norms (When controlling for the interaction effect of assortment sustainability and social norms.). b Interaction effect of distribution sustainability and personal norms (When controlling for the interaction effect of distribution sustainability and social norms.). c Interaction effect of assortment sustainability and social norms (When controlling for the interaction effect of assortment sustainability and personal norms.). d Interaction effect of distribution sustainability and social norms (When controlling for the interaction effect of distribution sustainability and personal norms.). Notes n.s. slope is not significantly different from zero. We display the interaction effects on consumers’ identification with the store. For store legitimacy, the effects have comparable patterns (significance and slope direction); the magnitudes of the slopes of the interaction effects and the absolute predicted values for legitimacy are different

In contrast to H5, when controlling for moderation by personal norms, social norms weakened (rather than strengthened) the effects of assortment sustainability on consumers’ identification with the store (b = −.13, p < .01) and store legitimacy (b = −.19, p < .01). The interaction between social norms and distribution sustainability on identification was also negative and significant (b = −.06, p < .05), whereas the effect on legitimacy was not significant (b = −.09, p > .05). That is, we did not find support for H6 (see Fig. 2, Panels C and D).Footnote 2

Discussion

Theoretical Implications

This study has aimed to contribute to an improved understanding of how and when retailers’ sustainability efforts translate into positive consumer responses. To that end, we examined personal and social mechanisms underlying consumer responses to retail stores’ sustainability efforts and thereby responded to calls for research, as issued by Bhattacharya et al. (2009), Yoon et al. (2006) and Vitell (2015). Our mediation and moderation analyses showed several interesting findings, which extend prior research and advance the marketing literature.

Our mediation analyses showed that consumers’ positive responses to retailers’ sustainability efforts largely occur through two routes: a personal route, via consumers’ identification with the retail store, and a social route, through store legitimacy. These routes are complementary, which implies that future research on consumer responses to sustainability efforts should consider both mechanisms simultaneously instead of just one or the other, which has been the case in prior studies (Handelman and Arnold 1999; Lichtenstein et al. 2004; Sen and Bhattacharya 2001). The two routes encompass benefits at different levels: the consumer level and retailer level. Retailers’ sustainability efforts as such may directly benefit consumers (and society at large), for example by minimizing exhaust emissions in city centers. In addition, our findings suggest that retailers’ sustainability efforts enable consumers to identify with retailers. Such a perception to be one with or belong to an organization that does good can trigger positive psychological effects, because it contributes to consumers’ self-esteem by allowing them to define themselves as people who do good (Ashforth and Mael 1989; Bergami and Bagozzi 2000). Hence, retailers’ sustainability efforts may have positive effects, both physical and psychological, on consumer well-being.

The route via store legitimacy shows that retailers that make sustainability efforts are perceived as acting in line with institutional norms, which, according to institutional theory, is crucial for organizational survival (Meyer and Rowan 1977). Without legitimacy, organizations are unable to obtain the necessary support from stakeholders, including investors, customers, and employees (Zimmerman and Zeitz 2002). Therefore, legitimacy is of direct benefit to the retailer.

We found that the personal route through identification led to much stronger effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses than the social route via legitimacy, which highlights the important role personal factors play in consumer responses to sustainability efforts. A similar pattern of results occurred for both assortment sustainability and distribution sustainability. However, as suggested by Peloza and Shang (2011), different sustainability efforts might have differential effects. We suggest that future research on consumer responses to sustainability efforts examines potential differences not only in ultimate outcomes of sustainability efforts, but also in the mechanisms that lead to those outcomes. Some efforts might exert their influence more strongly through the personal route, whereas others might largely follow the social route. In addition, these routes might interact.

After controlling for the indirect (positive) effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses via identification and legitimacy, we also detected some direct effects that remained. Baron and Kenny (1986, p. 1176) note that such direct effects indicate “the operation of multiple mediating factors,” and Zhao et al. (2010, p. 199) contend that “such direct paths often result from omission of one or more mediators from the model” and that “the sign of the mysterious ‘direct’ effect has heuristic value for theory building.” The sign of a remaining direct effect indicates which sign the additional mediator should have, but we found both positive and negative remaining direct effects. Therefore, it appears that at least two additional mediators are operating, one with a positive sign and another with a negative sign. Existing research suggests that customer satisfaction (Luo and Bhattacharya 2006) or perceived value (Peloza and Shang 2011) might be potential positive mediators of the sustainability efforts–consumer responses relationship; consumer skepticism might be a likely negative mediator. The number of companies that claim to operate in a socially responsible or sustainable way has increased dramatically in the past two decades (e.g., Adweek2009), while incidents of irresponsible behaviors such as greenwashing simultaneously have rendered consumers skeptical of such claims (Cronin et al. 2011; Mohr et al. 1998; Wagner et al. 2009). Alternatively, consumers’ perceived organizational motives (i.e., attributions) might be potential positive and negative mediators of the sustainability efforts–consumer responses relationship. Sustainability efforts might induce both positive (values-driven and strategic-driven) attributions and negative (stakeholder-driven) attributions, which in turn affect consumer responses such as purchase intentions (Groza et al. 2011).

In addition to how sustainability efforts translate into positive consumer responses, we also examined when it occurs, i.e., under which conditions. We expected that consumers’ positive responses would be enhanced by two conditions, namely if consumers (1) had strong personal norms and (2) felt strong social norms, both in favor of shopping at environmentally friendly stores. The effects of personal norms were as hypothesized, except for the interaction effect of distribution sustainability and personal norms on store legitimacy, which was not significant. Figure 2, Panels A and B show that consumers with weak personal norms do not respond or even respond negatively in terms of identification (and legitimacy) to retailers’ sustainability efforts, whereas consumers with average or strong personal norms respond positively. These findings contribute to an improved understanding of the boundaries and opportunities of sustainability efforts.

Remarkably, the effects of social norms were opposite to what we expected: When we controlled for moderation by personal norms, the effects of assortment sustainability on identification and legitimacy, and the effect of distribution sustainability on identification were weakened by social norms. This result contradicts most prior research on social norms (e.g., Goldstein et al. 2008; Smith et al. 2008); reactance theory may provide an explanation. That is, people believe that they have behavioral freedoms (Brehm and Mann 1975; Woller et al. 2007), such as the freedom to respond to sustainability efforts as they prefer. When this freedom appears threatened, such as by social pressure to perform a certain behavior (beyond the person’s personal norms), it may invoke feelings of reactance, “a motivational state that is directed toward the restoration of whatever freedom has been threatened or eliminated” (Brehm and Mann 1975, p. 816). Threatened freedom can be restored by resistance to social pressure.

In the context of our study, social pressure to shop at environmentally friendly stores may arouse reactance and cause consumers to respond less positively or not respond at all in terms of identification and legitimacy to retailers’ sustainability efforts. The dramatic increase in attention to sustainability in recent years may have created sustainability overkill: Our findings suggest that consumers do not want to be told that they should shop at environmentally friendly stores. Instead, they seem to prefer to decide for themselves how to behave, which is in line with our finding that personal norms strengthen the effects of sustainability efforts on identification and legitimacy. These results imply that the role of the social environment in consumer responses to sustainability efforts may diverge from prevailing views on the effect of social milieu on consumer behavior (e.g., Ajzen 1991).

Managerial Implications

We demonstrate that retailers with a sustainable assortment and a sustainable distribution achieve more positive store evaluations, stronger shopping intentions and word-of-mouth intentions, and a higher willingness to pay more by consumers than retailers that do not have a sustainable assortment or distribution. These findings show that traditional marketing mix elements provide opportunities for retailers to improve their organizations’ bottom line and positively affect consumer and societal well-being. For example, the Binnenstadservice initiative aims to reduce the amount of freight traffic in city centers by consolidating goods from multiple retailers in a distribution center outside the city center. Instead of multiple suppliers entering the city center with small shipments, only the Binnenstadservice truck enters the city center to distribute the goods, which leads to promising results in terms of reduced inconvenience for residents, improved traffic safety, and a more attractive shopping environment (Van Rooijen and Quak 2010). By participating in the Binnenstadservice initiative, retailers have the potential to improve the quality of life of city center residents and shoppers and to distinguish themselves from retailers that do not participate (until the point that all retailers participate). Furthermore, sustainability efforts allow consumers to identify with retailers that do good, which likely contributes to their self-esteem. It thus may pay off for retailers to make consumers aware of their sustainability efforts. Retailers might highlight their sustainable products and business practices through company websites and in-store information displays, though research suggests that their communication strategies must take care to avoid consumer perceptions of company hypocrisy (Wagner et al. 2009) or insincere motives (Yoon et al. 2006). Our study shows that the effects of retailers’ sustainability efforts on consumer responses are mediated by consumers’ identification with the store and store legitimacy. In addition to the positive consequences of identification and legitimacy on store evaluations, shopping intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more, these concepts might have other positive effects. Morgan and Hunt (1994), for example, show (in a business-to-business context) that shared values, which are the basis for organizational identification, improve customer trust and relationship commitment, which in turn lead to a range of positive outcomes. Sustainability efforts that improve organizational identification may provide retailers with the opportunity to build relationships with their customers. In addition, these efforts contribute to an organization’s legitimacy, which is crucial for organizational survival (Zimmerman and Zeitz 2002).

The effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses grow stronger due to personal norms; when we controlled for such norms, the effects were weakened by social norms. Therefore, managers must take care with the message in their sustainability communications. In trying to convince consumers to shop at sustainable stores, retailers should avoid referring to norms held by important others (e.g., “Don’t you think your friends would want you to shop responsibly?”) and focus instead on consumers’ personal norms. Consumers are motivated to behave consistently with their personal norms, because doing so results in positive self-evaluations, such as pride or self-esteem (Schwartz 1977). By emphasizing positive self-evaluations (e.g., “Shop with pride. Shop responsibly”), retailers can activate personal norms and prompt more favorable consumer responses to sustainability efforts. In addition, the insights of this study can help retailers decide whether or not to invest in and communicate about sustainability. For example, if a retailer knows that its target customers have weak personal norms regarding shopping at environmentally friendly stores, it may decide not to invest in sustainability or at least not to actively communicate about it to its customers, because they tend to respond negatively.

Although from a marketing perspective it is clear that the implications above can benefit retailers, from an ethical perspective the question can be asked whether informing companies how they can manipulate consumer behavior in order to increase the effectiveness of their sustainability efforts is ethical. According to Beauchamp’s (2001, cited by Villarán 2015) “continuum of influences”, companies can use advertising in three ways to make people buy a product, or in this case shop at sustainable stores: by using rational persuasion, coercion, or manipulation. Manipulative advertising in turn can be divided into deceptive and irrational advertising (Villarán 2015). It is generally agreed upon that advertising should not be deceptive, i.e., bringing about consumer misconceptions using false claims, important omissions, or misrepresentations of what the facts mean (Sher 2011). However, irrational advertising (playing on people’s vulnerabilities instead of their reason) is different: It is not illegal, commonly used, and accepted by many (Villarán 2015). Still, irrational advertising can be criticized.

Most importantly, irrational advertising threatens people’s moral autonomy as theorized by Kant. Kant (1999[1788], cited by Villarán 2015, p. 7), defines autonomy of the will as “the sole principle of all moral laws and duties in keeping with them… That is to say, the sole principle of morality consists in independence from all matter of the law (namely, from a desired object) and at the same time in the determination of choice through the mere form of giving universal law that a maxim must be capable of.” Accordingly, the critique on irrational advertising can be described as “to manipulate people is not to treat them as ends—certainly not as autonomous legislating members of a kingdom of ends. (…) it is something that we prefer not to happen to us and therefore shall not will it as a universal maxim” (Hare 1984, p. 28). So even if the goal of irrational advertising is to increase sustainable consumer behavior and sustainability is generally regarded as something positive, the action of manipulating consumers is wrong in itself, regardless of its consequencesFootnote 3 (Villarán 2015). The same applies to informing companies how they can manipulate consumers by appealing to social or personal norms: the end does not justify the means.

However, not all advertising is wrong in the sense that it threatens moral autonomy. If we recall Beauchamp’s “continuum of influences,” the positive extreme of the continuum consists of rational persuasion: “A deliberative and successful attempt by one person to encourage another to freely accept beliefs, attitudes, values, or actions through appeals to reason” (Beauchamp 2001, p. 477, cited by Villarán 2015, p. 2). So instead of appealing to personal norms, retailers could use rational advertising to inform consumers about the consequences of shopping at sustainable stores. For example, the factual effects of a sustainable assortment compared to an unsustainable assortment on the environment can be advertised. Such advertising could increase consumers’ awareness of consequences, which in turn might influence consumer behavior as well as consumers’ personal norms. The difference is that this influence is now based on appeals to reason instead of people’s vulnerabilities.

Finally, the question can be asked whether it is commercial companies’ responsibility to change consumer behavior, in this case, to persuade consumers to make shopping decisions based on sustainability. We argue that this is not necessarily companies’ responsibility, but as Vitell (2015) shows, for Corporate Social Responsibility to be successful, Consumer Social Responsibility is required. So it will lead to commercial success if companies can convince consumers to shop based on sustainability. In fact, one could go even one step further and argue that it is indeed CSR-focused companies’ responsibility to convince consumers to make shopping decisions based on social responsibility/sustainability, because only then they can maximize their beneficial impact on society.

Limitations and Suggestions for Further Research

With our experimental approach, we manipulated two specific sustainability efforts in the context of two store types and gathered deeper insights into how and when retailers’ sustainability efforts translate into positive consumer responses. However, there are also several limitations inherent to our method, which offer suggestions for further research.

First, we used a scenario-based approach, which limits the external validity of our findings. Although we tried to make the scenarios as credible and realistic as possible, scenarios are always simplified, limited versions of reality. Most consumers probably take into account various store attributes, such as store atmosphere and friendliness of store personnel, beyond sustainability, when evaluating a retailer and choosing where to shop (Pan and Zinkhan 2006). Furthermore, actual consumer awareness of companies’ sustainability efforts is generally low, which hinders companies’ abilities to reap rewards from their efforts (Sen et al. 2006). To increase external validity, further research should examine consumer responses to sustainability efforts implemented by a real company in a real-life situation. In addition, the effects of consumer awareness of and familiarity with companies’ sustainability efforts, as well as the influence of consumers’ sociodemographic characteristics on consumer responses to sustainability efforts, should be investigated.

Second, though our research provides a nuanced view of consumer responses to retailers’ sustainability efforts, we measured behavioral intentions (shopping intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and willingness to pay more) instead of actual behavior. Intentions do not automatically translate into behavior (Ajzen 1991). For example, many consumers say they are willing to buy green products, but only a small percentage of consumers do so (Luchs et al. 2010). Therefore, additional research should focus on actual behavior.

Third, two of the four legitimacy scale items were deleted during the evaluation of our measurement model. Although this step was necessary to obtain discriminant validity of the legitimacy and identification constructs, the question might arise whether the meaning of the legitimacy scale has changed. In order to arrive at the decision to delete two items, we carefully looked at the definition of the construct and concluded that these two items could be interpreted as measuring something other than perceptions of appropriateness within some socially constructed system of norms. So by deleting these items, we believe that the scale better reflects the legitimacy construct as we defined it in our study (with a focus on the moral aspects of legitimacy). However, two-item scales might pose problems in terms of not representing the entire content domain of the construct (MacKenzie et al. 2011). Therefore, we do suggest further research on the conceptual definition of legitimacy and its measurement (including formal scale development procedures, see, e.g., MacKenzie et al. 2011), in particular in the context of sustainability, because it influences the extent to which the pragmatic and moral components of legitimacy are relevant.

Fourth, our findings suggest the need to examine additional mediators (both positive and negative) to explain the direct effects of sustainability efforts on consumer responses. Potential positive mediators include customer satisfaction (Luo and Bhattacharya 2006), perceived value (Peloza and Shang 2011) or positive (values-driven or strategic-driven) attributions (Groza et al. 2011); negative mediators might be consumer skepticism (Cronin et al. 2011; Mohr et al. 1998; Wagner et al. 2009) or negative (stakeholder-driven) attributions (Groza et al. 2011). It would also be interesting to test reactance theory as a potential explanation for our finding that (when controlling for moderation by personal norms) subjective norms negatively moderate the effects of store assortment sustainability on consumer responses.