Skip to main content
Log in

Perception of Interannual Covariation and Strategies for Risk Reduction among Mikea of Madagascar

Individual and Social Learning

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper begins with the hypothesis that Mikea, participants in a mixed foraging–fishing–farming–herding economy of southwestern Madagascar, may attempt to reduce interannual variance in food supply caused by unpredictable rainfall by following a simple rule-of-thumb: Practice an even mix of activities that covary positively with rainfall and activities that covary negatively with rainfall. Results from a historical matrix participatory exercise confirm that Mikea perceive that foraging and farming outcomes covary positively or negatively with rainfall. This paper further considers whether Mikea learn about covariation through personal observation and memory recall (individual learning) or through socially transmitted ethnotheory (social learning). Dual inheritance theory models by Boyd and Richerson (1988) predict that individual learning is more effective in spatially and temporally variable environments such as the Mikea Forest. In contrast, the psychological literature suggests that individuals judge covariation poorly when memory of past events is required, unless they share a socially learned theory that a covariation should exist (Nisbett and Ross 1980). Results suggest that Mikea rely heavily on shared ethnotheory when judging covariation, but individuals continually strive to improve their judgment through individual observation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1988). An evolutionary model of social learning: The effects of spatial and temporal variation. In T. R. Zentall & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 29–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2001). Norms and bounded rationality. In G. Gigerenzer & R. Selten (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox (pp. 281–296). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E. (1992). Spatial organization and habitat use. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 237–266). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, L. J. (1967). Illusory correlation in observational report. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 151–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fanony, F. (1986). A propos des Mikea. In C. P. Kottak, J.-A. Rakotoarisoa, A. Southall & P. Vérin, (Eds.), Madagascar: Society and history (pp. 133–142). Durham: Carolina Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freudenberger, K. S. (1994). Tree and land tenure: Rapid rural appraisal tools. Community Forest Field Manual 4. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.

  • Freudenberger, K. S. (1996) The use of Rra to inform policy: Observations from Madagascar and Guinea. PLA Notes 26: Participation, Policy, and Institutionalization. University of Sussex.

  • Harpending, H., & Davis, H. (1977). Some implications for hunter–gatherer ecology derived from the spatial structure of resources. World Archaeology, 8, 275–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, R. P., & Riegelman, R. K. (1992). Statistical first aid: Interpretation of health data. Washington: Blackwell Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunn, E. S., Johnson, D., Russell, P., & Thornton, T. F. (2003). Huna Tlingit traditional environmental knowledge, conservation, and the management of a “wilderness” park. Current Anthropology, 44, s79–s103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958) The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johannes, R. E. (1989). Traditional environmental knowledge: A collection of essays. Cambridge: World Conservation Union.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz, H. (1952). Portfolio selection. Journal of Finance, 7, 77–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgement. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orlove, B. S., Chiang, J. C. H., & Cane, M. A. (2002). Ethnoclimatology in the Andes. American Scientist, 90, 428–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H., & Mims, M. (1981). Development of rule use in judgments of covariation between events. Child Development, 52, 317–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H., & Mims, M. (1982). Sources of error in judging event covariations: Effects of memory demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 208–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaklee, H., & Tucker, D. (1980). A rule analysis of judgments of covariation between events. Memory & Cognition, 8, 459–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokal, R. R., & Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorg, J.-P. (1996). Vernacular and scientific names of plants of the Morondava region. Primate Report, 46(1), 339–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, B. (2001). The behavioral ecology and economics of variation, risk, and diversification among Mikea forager–farmers of Madagascar. Ph.D. dissertation, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

  • Tucker, B. (2003). Mikea origins: Relicts or refugees? Michigan Discussions in Anthropology, 14, 193–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, B., & Young, A. G. (2005). Growing up Mikea: Children’s time allocation and tuber foraging in Southwestern Madagascar. In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter–gatherer childhoods (pp. 147–171). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, W. D., & Jenkins, H. M. (1965). The display of information and the judgment of contingency. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19, 231–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1986). Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5, 369–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B. (1990). Open field, common pot: Harvest variability and risk avoidance in agricultural and foraging societies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 67–87). Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (1992). Natural selection and decision-making: Some fundamental principles. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 25–60). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. C. (1999). Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: Coping with an island environment. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 42, 31–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yount, J. W., Tsiazonera, & Tucker, B. (2001). Constructing Mikea identity: Past or present links to forest and foraging. Ethnohistory, 48, 257–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

Several individuals deserve thanks for the roles they played in the conception of this paper. As part of his dissertation research, James Yount arranged for Karen Freudenberger to train our Malagasy colleagues Tsiazonera and Jaovola Tombo in participatory methods including the historical matrix in Fianarantsoa, Madagascar, in 1998. During my dissertation follow-up project in 1999, Tsiazonera and Jaovola taught me the historical matrix exercise, and we decided that it could be used to reveal covariation perception. They, along with Gervais Tantely and Tsimitamby, assisted with data collection. Special thanks to all our research participants for their patience. HASYMA kindly provided data from their weather stations and official permission to publish these data. Thanks to the following for comments on previous drafts: James Yount, Larry Kuznar, Joe Henrich, Lexine Trask, Tammy Watkins, Lisa Chaudhari, and the anonymous reviewers. Field research was funded by a National Science Foundation Dissertation Improvement Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bram Tucker.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tucker, B. Perception of Interannual Covariation and Strategies for Risk Reduction among Mikea of Madagascar. Hum Nat 18, 162–180 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9007-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9007-z

Keywords

Navigation