Skip to main content
Log in

Epistemological discipline in animal behavior studies: Konrad Lorenz and Daniel Lehrman on intuition and empathy

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Can empathy be a tool for obtaining scientific knowledge or is it incompatible with the detached objectivity that is often seen as the ideal in scientific inquiry? This paper examines the views of Austrian ethologist Konrad Lorenz and American comparative psychologist Daniel Lehrman on the role of intuition and empathy in the study of animal behavior. It situates those views within the larger project of establishing ethology as an objective science. Lehrman challenged Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen, the main founders of this field, to clarify their epistemological positions regarding how to deal with the subjectivity of the animals they studied as well as the scientist’s own subjectivity. I argue that there was a tension between their desire to eliminate the subjectivities of ethological researchers (and of their subjects) and the public perception that Lorenz had a remarkable ability to enter into the lives of the animals he studied. I explain why Lorenz rejected empathy as valid in scientific inquiry, showing that his epistemological position was grounded in his ideal of science and his proposed ontology for ethology. Yet, Lehrman insisted that full detachment was neither possible nor desirable.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For reasons of space, I do not cover here their scientific practices. Further work on this area will be needed to explore whether their actual observation practices and experiments exemplified their positions about the role of empathy in epistemology and methodology or whether they departed from their programmatic claims.

  2. On the history of ethology, see Burkhardt et al. (1997), Burkhardt (2005) and Dewsbury (1985). On the ethologists’ views about human behavior, see Vicedo (2013, 2018a, 2018b), Milam (2019) and Weidman (2021). On Lorenz’s life, see Taschwer & Föger, (2003). For philosophical examination of Lorenz’s ideas, see Griffiths (2004) and Brigandt (2005). On Tinbergen’s life and work, see Kruuk (2003), Röell (2000) and Lundl (2015). On Karl von Frisch, see Munz (2016). Over the years, ethology evolved in ways that departed from what is what called “classical ethology.” See Gräfe and Stuhrmann (2022) on the evolution of different approaches and theoretical premises within ethology over time. Here I focus on Lorenz’s and Tinbergen’s views during the period of classical ethology.

  3. For an extended criticism of Bierens de Haan’s views, see Lorenz (1942). Lorenz also rejected vitalism, including the position of Jakob von Uexküll (1864–1944), whose notion of Umwelt had influenced him (Burkhardt, 2005; Jaroš and Brentari, 2022).

  4. On Lorenz’s and Tinbergen’s views about the subjectivity of animals, see Burkhardt et al. (1997), Gorokhovskaya (2005). See also Klassen (2021); my views depart from her interpretation.

  5. On the three stages of scientific development and the role of intuition and induction, see also Lorenz (1948, pp. 30, 54; 1951, p. 176). See Brigandt (2003) for an analysis of Lorenz’s epistemological views.

  6. On different notions of observation in the sciences, see Daston and Lunbeck (2011).

  7. Gräfe (2022, pp. 69–70) notes that Lorenz not only started his own photo and film collection in 1935, but he also promoted the institutionalization of an international film collection for comparative ethological studies, the Encyclopaedia Cinematographica, initiated in 1952 at the Institute for the Scientific Film (IWF) in Göttingen. On the significance of film for the early ethological studies, see also Mitman (1999), Burkhardt (2005), Munz (2005) and Scholz (2021). On animals in film more generally, see Burt (2003).

  8. On changing notions of objectivity, see also Megill (1994).

  9. On the construction of a scientific persona, see Daston and Sibum (2003).

  10. On Lorenz’s use of Martina to represent different ideas over time, see Munz (2011). On the significance of naming animals, see Benson (2016).

  11. On anthropomorphism, see Daston and Mitman (2005), Davis and Balfour (1992), Dwyer (2007), Haraway (2003, 2008), Mitchell et al. (1997), and Rees (2001).

  12. CBS Adventure Series, Animal Communication and Behavior, 23 January 1955. Available through The American Museum of Natural History Film Archives.

  13. “He talks baby ducks into thinking he’s mother,” New York Herald Tribune, 23 January 1955; “An adopted mother goose,” Life, 39 (July/August 1955), pp. 73–78.

  14. In her analysis of German sources, Kaufmann (2018, p. 30) has written that: “The general readers appreciated the scientist Lorenz precisely because of his empathetic closeness to animals and his depictions of their emotional and cognitive abilities” (my translation).

  15. Vicedo (2009, 2013) has shown that Lorenz’s work on imprinting played a key role in the development of the psychoanalytic conception of the good mother as the natural mother who could respond empathically to her children in an instinctive way. Lorenz’s studies on imprinting influenced the work of John Bowlby, René Spitz, Therese Benedek and many other psychoanalytic students of the mother–child relationship.

  16. Dewsbury (1997, p. 376) notes that in his popular writings “Tinbergen expressed his analysis with a sense of empathy that conveyed concern for, and frustration with, the birds’ behavior, rather than merely reporting his observations.”.

  17. In her study of field primatology, Rees (2007, p. 886) also highlights this tension:

    “A potential contradiction is evident between the researcher’s goal of physically integrating with the group and being treated by the other primates as ‘just’ another animal, and that of producing accounts of ‘natural’ behaviour unaffected by human interference.” She goes on to show how “strong emotional and empathic identification with the animals is consistently evident in their popular writings.” This, she argues, is especially the case in the work of Jane Goodall, Birutė Galdikas and Dian Fossey (p. 889).

  18. On Lehrman, see Rosenblatt (1995), Burkhardt (2005) and Vicedo (2013, 2023).

  19. On the history of empathy in psychoanalysis, see Pigman (1995), Richmond (2004), Lunbeck (2011) and Lanzoni (2018).

  20. The idea of ‘participant observation’ as a scientific method is central to discussions of modern anthropology, and variously attributed to Frank Hamilton Cushing and Bronislav Malinowski. The same issues of empathy and tension between total objectification and identification with the subject are discussed in the literature on anthropological methodology. See Baker (1987) for Malinowski, and LaCapra (2019), ch. 4 for a more recent discussion of these issues focused on Cushing.

  21. See Lorenz (1959, 1983) about his views on Gestalt perception as a source of scientific knowledge.

  22. Tinbergen claimed to have tried to instill pre-analytical intuition into his students, in a way reminiscent of Lorenz’s argument: “What I have always tried to teach my boys … is an approach from the intuitive-gestalt-observing side, which one then later analyses into components.” Tinbergen to John Newson, 29 March 1973, folder D.5, Nikolaas Tinbergen Papers (henceforth, Tinbergen Papers), NCUACS 27.3.91, Department of Western Manuscripts, Bodleian Library, University of Oxford.

  23. Today, too, the authority of many animal researchers, at least among the general public, derives not from their detachment but from their intimate associations with the animals they study, as Crist (1999, p. 176) and Mitman (2005), among others, have noted.

  24. Several of Lorenz’s writings in the 1950s and 1960s were about the methods of researching animal behavior and the issue of the animal’s subjective experience. See Lorenz (19541959, 1963).

  25. Niko Tinbergen to Stroh, 5 December 1970, folder D.1, Tinbergen Papers.

  26. This is how Lorenz told René Spitz he was called, and how Spitz introduced Lorenz in a conference. In “Introduction,” p. 1, Box M2116, Folder 7 (“Lorenz 1970”), René Spitz Papers, Archives of the History of American Psychology, Akron, OH.

References

  • Baker, V. (1987). Pitching a tent in the native village: Malinowski and participant observation. Bijdragen tot de Taal-, Land-en Volkenkunde, 143, 14–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beer, C. G. (1980). Perspectives on animal behaviour comparisons. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Comparative methods in psychology (pp. 17–64). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2007). The emotional lives of animals: A leading scientist explores animal joy, sorrow, and empathy, and why they matter. New World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, E. S. (2016). Naming the ethological subject. Science in Context, 29, 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, S. (1976). For God’s sake, Margaret: Conversation with Gregory Bateson and Margaret Mead. CoEvolution Quarterly, 10(21), 32–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brigandt, I. (2003). Gestalt experiments and inductive observations: Konrad Lorenz’s early epistemological writings and the methods of classical ethology. Evolution and Cognition, 9, 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brigandt, I. (2005). The instinct concept of the early Konrad Lorenz. Journal of the History of Biology, 38, 571–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Pettifer, J. (1985). The nature watchers: Exploring wildlife with the experts. Guild Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucchi, M. (1996). When scientists turn to the public: Alternative routes in science communication. Public Understanding of Science, 5, 375–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt, R. W., et al. (1997). The founders of ethology and the problem of animal subjective experience. In M. Dol (Ed.), Animal consciousness and animal ethics (pp. 1–13). Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt, R. W. (2005). Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, J. (2003). Animals in film. Reaktion Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Candea, M. (2010). ‘I fell in love with Carlos the meerkat’: Engagement and detachment in human–animal relations. American Ethnologist, 37(2), 241–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooter, R., & Pumfrey, S. (1994). Separate spheres and public places: Reflections on the history of science popularisation and science in popular culture. History of Science, 32, 237–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crist, E. (1999). Images of animals: Anthropomorphism and animal mind. Temple University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Galison, P. (2007). Objectivity. Zone Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Lunbeck, E. (Eds.). (2011). Histories of scientific observation. Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Mitman, G. (Eds.). (2005). Thinking with animals. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L., & Sibum, H. O. (2003). Introduction: Scientific personae and their histories. Science in Context, 16(1/2), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H., & Balfour, D. (1992). The inevitable bond: Examining scientist-animal interactions. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, O. L., Yeager, E. A., & Foster, S. J. (Eds.). (2001). Historical empathy and perspective taking in the social studies. Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (2009). The age of empathy: Nature’s lessons for a kinder society. Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (Ed.). (1985). Studying animal behavior: Autobiographies of the founders. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (1997). Rhetorical strategies in the presentation of ethology and comparative psychology in magazines after World War II. Science in Context, 10, 367–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. A. (2013). John B. Watson’s early work and comparative psychology. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 39(2), 10–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durant, J. R. (1981). Innate character in animals and man: A perspective on the origins of ethology. In C. Webster (Ed.), Biology, medicine and society 1840–1940 (pp. 157–192). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dwyer, J. (2007). A non-companion species manifesto: Humans, wild animals, and ‘the pain of anthropomorphism.’ South Atlantic Review, 72(3), 73–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. H. (2013). A brief conceptual history of Einfühlung: 18th-century Germany to post-World War II U.S. psychology. History of Psychology, 16, 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, P. (1997). Identification and economic behavior: Sympathy and empathy in historical perspective. Economics and Philosophy, 13(2), 261–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, P. (2001). The changing place of empathy in welfare economics. History of Political Economy, 33(3), 387–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gräfe, S. (2022). Red foxes in the filing cabinet: Günter Tembrock’s image collection and media use in mid-century ethology. Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, 45(1), 55–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gräfe, S., & Stuhrmann, C. (2022). Histories of ethology: Methods, sites, and dynamics of an unbound discipline. Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte, 45(1), 10-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, J., & Miller, S. (1998). Science in public: Communication, culture and credibility. Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. (1992). Animal minds. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. E. (2004). Instinct in the ‘50s: The British reception of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 609–631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorokhovskaya, E. (2005). Subjective animal world and objectivistic Konrad Lorenz’s ethology. Entomological Review, 85, S34–S41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. (1990). Primate visions: Gender, race and nature in the world of modern science. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. (2003). The companion species manifesto: Dogs, people, and significant otherness. Prickly Paradigm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. (2008). When species meet. University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgartner, S. (1990). The dominant view of popularisation: Conceptual problems, political uses. Social Studies of Science, 20, 519–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaroš, F., & Brentari, C. (2022). Organisms as subjects: Jakob von Uexküll and Adolf Portmann on the autonomy of living beings and anthropological difference. HPLS, 44, 36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-022-00518-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, D. (2018). Konrad Lorenz: Scientific persona, ‘Harnack-Pläncker’ und Wissenschaftsstar in der Zeit des Kalten Krieges bis in die frühen 1970er Jahre. Preprint, Ergebnisse des Forschungsprogramms Geschichte der Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. https://pure.mpg.de/rest/items/item_3010668_5/component/file_3081258/content

  • Klassen, A. (2021). Methodological signatures in early ethology: The problem of animal subjectivity. Journal for General Philosophy of Science/Zeitschrift für Allgemeine Wissenschaftstheorie, 52(4), 563–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koegler, H. H., & Stueber, K. R. (Eds.). (2000). Empathy and agency: The problem of understanding in the human sciences. Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruuk, H. (2003). Niko’s nature: The life of Niko Tinbergen and his science of animal behaviour. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaCapra, D. (2019). Understanding others: Peoples, animals, pasts. Cornell University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lanzoni, S. (2018). Empathy: A history. Yale University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 337–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1955a). The perception of animal behavior. In B. Schaffner (Ed.), (1955) Group processes (pp. 259–267). Madison Printing Co.

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1955b). The physiological basis of parental feeding behavior in the ring dove (Streptopelia risoria). Behaviour, 7, 241–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1964). The reproductive behavior of ring doves. Scientific American, 211, 48–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1971). Behavioral science, engineering and poetry”. In E. Tobach, L. R. Aronson, & E. Shaw (Eds.), The biopsychology of development (pp. 459–471). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightman, B. (2007). Victorian popularizers of science: Designing nature for new audiences. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1932). A consideration of the methods of identification of species-specific instinctive behaviour patterns. In K. Lorenz (Ed.) (1970), Studies in animal and human behavior (Vol. I, pp. 57–100). Harvard University Press.

  • Lorenz, K. (1935). Companionship in bird life: Fellow members of the species as releasers of social behavior. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.) (1957), Instinctive behavior (pp. 83–128). International Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1937). The nature of instinct. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.) (1957), Instinctive behavior (pp. 129–175). International Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1942). Inductive and teleological psychology. In K. Lorenz (Ed.) (1970), Studies in animal and human behavior, Vol. I, (pp. 351–370). Harvard University Press.

  • Lorenz, K. (1948). The natural science of the human Species: An introduction to comparative research. The “Russian manuscript” (1944–1948). Edited from the author’s post-humous works by A. von Cranach in 1996. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1950). The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 221–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1951). The role of gestalt perception in animal and human behavior. In L. L. Whyte (Ed.), Aspects of form: A symposium on form in nature and art (pp. 157–178). Lund Humphries.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1952). King Solomon’s ring: New light on animal ways. Crowell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1953). Man meets dog. Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1954). Psychology and phylogeny. In K. Lorenz (Ed.) (1971), Studies in animal and human behavior, Vol. II, (pp. 196–245). Harvard University Press.

  • Lorenz, K., et al. (1956). The objectivistic theory of instinct. In M. Autuori (Ed.), L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l’homme (pp. 51–70). Masson et Cie.

  • Lorenz, K. (1959). Gestalt perception as a source of scientific knowledge. In K. Lorenz (Ed.) (1970), Studies in animal and human behavior, Vol. I, (pp. 281–322). Harvard University Press.

  • Lorenz, K. (1960). Methods of approach to the problems of behavior. In The Harvey lectures, 1958–1959 (pp. 60–103). Academic Press.

  • Lorenz, K. (1963). Do animals undergo subjective experience? In K. Lorenz (Ed.) (1970), Studies in animal and human behavior, Vol. I, (pp. 323–337). Harvard University Press.

  • Lorenz, K. (1969). On aggression. Bantam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1973). Autobiography. https://www.nobelprize.org/prizes/medicine/1973/lorenz/facts/.

  • Lorenz, K. (1978). The year of the greylag goose. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1983). The waning of humaneness. Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lunbeck, E. (2011). Empathy as a psychoanalytic mode of observation: Between sentiment and science in Daston & Lunbeck, eds (pp. 255–275). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundl, C. (2015). Drifting ethologists. Jacobs University Bremen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Megill, A. (Ed.). (1994). Rethinking objectivity. Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milam, E. (2019). Creatures of Cain: The hunt for human nature in Cold War America. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. W., Thompson, N. S., & Miles, H. L. (Eds.). (1997). Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals. State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitman, G. (1999). Reel nature: America’s romance with wildlife on film. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitman, G. (2005). Pachyderm personalities: The media of science, politics, and conservation. In Daston & Mitman (Eds.), Thinking with animals. New perspectives on anthropomorphism (pp. 175–195). Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, G. M. (2005). Place, practice and primatology: Clarence Ray Carpenter, primate communication and the development of field methodology, 1931–1945. Journal of the History of Biology, 38, 495–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munz, T. (2005). Die Ethologie des wissenschaflichen Cineasten: Karl von Frisch, Konrad Lorenz und das Verhalten der Tiere im Film. Montage/av, 14, 52–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munz, T. (2011). ‘My goose child Martina’: The multiple uses of geese in the writings of Konrad Lorenz. Historical Studies in the Natural Sciences, 41, 405–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munz, T. (2016). The dancing bees: Karl von Frisch and the discovery of the honeybee language. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, G. (1990). Writing biology: Texts in the social construction of scientific knowledge. University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pigman, G. (1995). Freud and the history of empathy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 76, 237–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, A. (2001). Anthropomorphism, anthropocentrism, and anecdote: Primatologists on primatology. Science, Technology, & Human Values, 26(2), 227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, A. (2007). Reflections on the field-primatology, popular science and the politics of personhood. Social Studies of Science, 37, 881–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richmond, S. (2004). Being in others: Empathy from a psychoanalytical perspective. European Journal of Philosophy, 12(2), 244–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Röell, D. R. (2000). Niko Tinbergen and the rise of ethology in the Netherlands (1920–1950). Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. C. (2020). In the hearts of the beasts. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, J. S. (1995). Daniel Sanford Lehrman, June 1, 1919-August 27, 1972. Biographical Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences, 66, 227–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffner, B. (Ed.). (1955). Group processes: Transactions of the First Conference, September 26-30, 1954, Ithaca, New York. Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffer, S. (1988). Astronomers mark time: Discipline and the personal equation. Science in Context, 2, 101–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneirla, T. C. (1950). The relation between observation and experimentation in the field study of behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 51, 1022–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz, J. (2021). Duplicating nature and elements of subjectivity in “The ethology of the greylag goose.” Isis, 112(2), 326–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taschwer, K., & Föger, B. (2003). Konrad Lorenz: Biographie. Zsolnay.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1942). An objectivistic study of the innate behaviour of animals. Bibliotheca Biotheoretica, Series D, 1, 39–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1973). The animal in its world: Explorations of an ethologist, 1932–1972. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J., & Morris, E. K. (1986). The early research of John B. Watson: Before the behavioral revolution. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 71–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2009). ‘The father of ethology and the foster mother of ducks’: Konrad Lorenz as an expert on motherhood. Isis, 100, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2013). The nature and nurture of love: From imprinting to attachment in Cold War America. University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2018a). The ‘disadapted’ animal: Niko Tinbergen on human nature and the human predicament. Journal of the History of Biology, 51(2), 191–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2018b). Ethopathology and civilization diseases: Niko and Elisabeth Tinbergen on autism. Canadian Bulletin of Medical History, 35(1), 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicedo, M. (2023). Beyond the instinct debate: Daniel Lehrman’s contributions to animal behavior studies. Journal of the History of Biology, 56 (1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10739-023-09706-y.

  • Watson, J. B. (1909). The behavior of noddy and sooty terns. Papers from the Tortugas Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, 2, 187–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weidman, N. (2021). Killer instinct: The popular science of human nature in twentieth-century America. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Richard W. Burkhardt, Juan Ilerbaig, Mark Solovey, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions to improve this paper.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marga Vicedo.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vicedo, M. Epistemological discipline in animal behavior studies: Konrad Lorenz and Daniel Lehrman on intuition and empathy. HPLS 45, 6 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-023-00558-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-023-00558-7

Keywords

Navigation