Skip to main content
Log in

Is egocentric bias evidence for simulation theory?

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Revised simulation theory (Goldman 2006) allows mental state attributions containing some or all of the attributor’s genuine, non-simulated mental states. It is thought that this gives the revised theory an empirical advantage, because unlike theory theory and rationality theory, it can explain egocentric bias (the tendency to over attribute ones’ own mental states to others). I challenge this view, arguing that theory theory and rationality theory can explain egocentricity by appealing to heuristic mindreading and the diagnosticity of attributors’ own beliefs, and that these explanations are as simple and consistent as those provided by revised simulation theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Camerer C., Loewenstein G., Weber M. (1989) The curse of knowledge in economic settings: An experimental analysis. Journal of Political Economy 97(5): 1232–1254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawes R. M. (1989) Statistical criteria for establishing a truly false consensus effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 25: 1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawes R. M. (1990) The potential nonfalsity of the false consensus effect. In: Hogarth R. M. (eds) Insights in decision making: A tribute to Hillel J. Einhorn. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 179–199

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawes R. M., Mulford M. (1996) The false consensus effect and overconfidence: Flaws in judgment or flaws in how we study judgment?. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 65(3): 201–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelmann D., Strobel M. (2000) The false consensus effect disappears if representative information and monetary incentives are given. Experimental Economics 3: 241–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff B. (1975) Hindsight ≠ foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1: 288–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff B., Beyth R. (1975) “I knew it would happen”—remembered probabilities of once-future things. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 13: 1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A. I. (2006) Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A. I., Sebanz N. (2005) Simulation, mirroring, and a different argument from error. Trends in Cognitive Science 9(7): 320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman G. H. (1965) The inference to the best explanation. The Philosophical Review 74(1): 88–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoch S. J. (1987) Perceived consensus and predicted accuracy: The pros and cons of projection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53: 221–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D., Slovic P., Tversky A. (Eds). (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D., Tversky A. (1984) Choices, values and frames. American Psychologist 39: 341–350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keysar B., Lin S., Barr D. J. (2003) Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition 89: 25–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss R. M., Glucksberg S. (1969) The development of communication: Competence as a function of age. Child Development 40: 256–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger J., Clement R. W. (1994) The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 596–610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn T. S. (1977) The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Newton, E. (1990). Overconfidence in the communication of intent: Heard and unheard melodies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.

  • Pronin E., Puccio C., Ross L. (2002) Understanding misunderstandings: Social psychological perspectives. In: Gilovich T., Griffin D., Kahneman D. (eds) Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 636–665

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross L., Greene D., House P. (1977) The ‘false consensus effect’: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 13: 279–301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R. (2005) Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends in Cognitive Science 9(4): 174–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven L., Dunning D., Loewenstein G. (2000) Egocentric empathy gaps between owners and buyers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79: 66–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven L., Loewenstein G. (2003) Social projection of transient drive states. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(9): 1159–1168

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annika Wallin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wallin, A. Is egocentric bias evidence for simulation theory?. Synthese 178, 503–514 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-009-9653-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-009-9653-2

Keywords

Navigation