Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Implicit Theories and Issue Characteristics as Determinants of Moral Awareness and Intentions

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals’ implicit theories that people’s character is fixed (entity theorist) versus malleable (incremental theorist) are associated with their holding beliefs that morality is primarily determined by fulfilling prescribed duties (duty-based morality) versus upholding basic rights of others (rights-based morality), respectively. Three studies provide evidence that the ability to recognize that a situation can legitimately be considered from a moral point of view (moral awareness) is interactively dependent upon the nature of perceivers’ implicit theories (and their associated schemas in the moral domain) and the extent to which the issue involves a violation that emphasizes a failure to fulfill a prescribed duty (duty-based violation) versus a failure to uphold a fundamental right of others (rights-based violation). The studies experimentally manipulated the characteristics of a hypothetical business situation to involve either a duty-based violation or a rights-based violation or no behavioral violation. In addition, Study 1 experimentally manipulated subjects’ implicit theories, while Studies 2 and 3 measured subjects’ chronically held implicit theories. Collectively, the studies provide consistent evidence that entity theorists have greater moral awareness than incremental theorists when considering situations involving duty-based violations, while incremental theorists have greater moral awareness than entity theorists when considering situations involving rights-based violations, and moral awareness is not significantly different for perceivers who are neither strongly entity nor incremental theorists, when considering situations involving duty- versus rights-based violations. Study 3 also found evidence of a moderated-mediated association between violation type and moral intentions, through moral awareness as a mediator, moderated by perceivers’ implicit theories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 1–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akaah, I. (1989). Differences in research ethics judgments between male and female marketing professionals. Journal of Business Ethics, 8, 375–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255–291). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., Pichert, J. W., & Shirey, L. L. (1983). Effects of the readers’ schema at different points in time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 271–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arlow, P. (1991). Characteristics in college students’ evaluations of business ethics and corporate responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics, 10, 63–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baier, K. (1958). The moral point of view: A rational basis for ethics. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2005). Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 40, 373–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, M. H. (2001). Judgment in managerial decision making (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beltramini, R. F., Peterson, R. A., & Kozmetsky, G. (1984). Concerns of college students regarding business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 3(3), 195–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentham, J. (1948). Introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. New York: Hafner. Original work published 1789.

    Google Scholar 

  • BEQ Author Guide. (2014). Retrieved July 1, 2014, from http://www.pdcnet.org/pdf/BEQ%20Author%20Guide.pdf.

  • Betz, M., O’Connell, L., & Shepard, J. M. (1989). Gender differences in productivity for unethical behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 8, 321–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, S., & Ugras, Y. (1998). Business students and ethics: a meta-analysis. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(11), 1117–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breusch, T. S., & Pagan, A. R. (1980). The Lagrange multiplier test and its applications to model specification in econometrics. Review of Economic Studies, 47(1), 239–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B., Hong, Y., & Li, Q. (2004). Dynamic entitativity: Perceiving groups as actors. In V. Yzerbyt, C. Judd, & O. Corneille (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: Perceived variability, entitativity, and essentialism (pp. 25–38). New York: PsychologyPress.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briggs, N. (2006). Estimation of the standard error and confidence interval of the indirect effect in multiple mediator models. Dissertation Abstracts International, 37, 475B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, K. D., Trevino, L. K., & Weaver, G. R. (2000). Moral awareness in business organizations: Influences of issue-related and social context factors. Human Relations, 53, 981–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, S., Hadi, A. S., & Price, B. (2000). Regression analysis by example (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Tong, Y. Y., & Fu, H. Y. (1997a). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 923–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1997). Justice from a Chinese perspective. In H. S. R. Kao & D. Sinha (Eds.), Asian perspectives on psychology (pp. 164–184). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997b). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, D. (1989). Organizational harm, legal condemnation and stakeholder retaliation: A typology, research agenda and application. Journal of Business Ethics, 8, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, D. (2000). The quest to improve the human condition: The first 1500 articles published in the Journal of Business Ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 26, 1–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craft, J. L. (2013). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 2004–2011. Journal of Business Ethics, 117, 221–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R., & MacKinnon, J. G. (1993). Estimation and inference in econometrics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dukerich, J. M., Waller, M. J., George, E., & Huber, G. P. (2000). Moral intensity and managerial problem solving. Journal of Business Ethics, 24, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995a). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995b). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dworkin, R. (1978). Taking rights seriously. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analytical framework using moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellingson, J. E., Smith, D. B., & Sackett, P. R. (2001). Investigating the influence of social desirability in personality factor structure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1989). Values from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In N. Eisenberg, J. Reykowski, & E. Staub (Eds.), Social and moral values: Individual and societal perspectives (pp. 3–22). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdley, C. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1993). Children’s implicit personality theories as predictors of their social judgments. Child Development, 64, 863–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrell, O. C., Gresham, L. G., & Fraedrich, J. (1989). A synthesis of ethical decision models for marketing. Journal of Macromarketing, 9(2), 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, S., & Hambrick, D. C. (1996). Strategic leadership: Top executives and their effects on organizations. St. Paul: West Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Beliefi attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. New York: Taylor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franke, G. R., Crown, D. F., & Spake, D. F. (1997). Differences in ethical perceptions of business practices: A social role theory perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(6), 920–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18, 233–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervey, B. M., Chiu, C.-Y., Hong, Y.-Y., & Dweck, C. (1999). Differential use of person information in decisions about guilt versus innocence: The role of implicit theories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 17–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gioia, D. A. (1992). Pinto fires and personal ethics: A script analysis of missed opportunities. Journal of Business Ethics, 11, 379–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, E. T., Schallert, D. L., Reynolds, R. E., & Radin, D. I. (1983). Reading in perspective: What Real cops and pretend burglars look for in a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 500–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, W. H. (2008). Econometric analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Cai, L. (2007). Using heteroskedasticity-consistent standard errors in OLS regression: An introduction and software implementation. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 709–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924–936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2012). Conditional process modeling: Using structural equation modeling to examine contingent causal processes. In G. R. Hancock & O. Mueller (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: A second course (2nd ed.). Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hays, R. D., Hayashi, T., & Stewart, A. L. (1989). A five-item measure of socially desirable response set. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 49, 629–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebert, P., Meslin, E. M., Dunn, E. V., Byrne, N., & Reid, S. R. (1990). Evaluating ethical sensitivity in medical students: Using vignettes as an instrument. Journal of Medical Ethics, 16, 141–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hemsley, G. D., & Marmurek, H. H. C. (1982). Person memory: The processing of consistent and inconsistent person information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 433–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. E. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & King, G. A. (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Information-processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kohlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social interaction (pp. 69–122). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinkley, D. V. (1977). Jackknifing in unbalanced situations. Technometrics, 19, 285–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y., Levy, S. R., & Chiu, C. (2001). The contribution of the lay theories approach to the study of groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 97–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. (1986). A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of Macro Marketing, 6, 5–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. (1965). Structural characteristics of norms. In I. D. Steiner & M. Fishbein (Eds.), Current studies in social psychology (pp. 301–309). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. A. (1981). The “self-disclosure” and “self-presentation” views of items response dynamics and personality scale validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 761–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. A., Germer, C. K., Efran, J. S., & Overton, W. F. (1988). Personality as a basis for theoretical predilections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 824–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16, 366–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. (2009). A social cognitive framework for examining moral awareness in managers and academics. Journal of Business Ethics, 84, 237–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Key, S., & Popkin, S. J. (1998). Integrating ethics into the strategic management process: Doing well by doing good. Management Decision, 36, 331–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutner, M. H., & Nachtsheim, C. J. (2004). Applied linear regression models. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, C. L. L. (2010). A step forward: Ethics education matters! Journal of Business Ethics, 92, 565–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levontin, L., Halperin, E., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Implicit theories block negative attributions about a longstanding adversary: The case of Israelis and Arabs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 670–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (2006). Lay theories and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9(1), 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Trait- versus process-focused social judgment. Social Cognition, 16, 151–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). The impact of children’s static versus dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype formation. Child Development, 70, 1163–1180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Modes of social thought: Person theories and social understanding. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 179–202). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2001). Static versus dynamic theories and the perception of groups: Different routes to different destinations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 156–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1421–1436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, J. S., & Ervin, L. H. (2000). Using heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors in the linear regression model. American Statistician, 54, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longenecker, J. G., McKinney, J. A., & Moore, C. F. (1989). Ethics in small business. Journal of Small Business Management, 27(1), 27–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, Y. P., Rechner, P. L., & Olson-Buchanan, J. P. (2005). Shaping ethical perceptions: An empirical assessment of the influence of business education, culture, and demographic factors. Journal of Business Ethics, 60, 341–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luthar, H. K., DiBattista, R., & Gautschi, T. (1997). Perception of what the ethical climate is and what is should be: The role of gender academic status and ethical education. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 205–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luthar, H. K., & Karri, R. (2005). Exposure to ethics education and the perception of linkage between organizational ethical the ethical behavior and business outcomes. Journal of Business Ethics, 61, 353–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, J. G., & White, H. (1985). Some heteroskedasticity consistent covariance matrix estimators with improved finite sample properties. Journal of Econometrics, 29, 305–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, D. R., & Pauli, K. P. (2002). The role of moral intensity in moral decision-making. Business and Society, 41, 84–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When mediation is moderated and moderation is mediated. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 852–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 1996–2003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59, 375–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ocasio, W. (1997). Towards an attention-based view of the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 18, 187–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okleshen, M., & Hoyt, R. (1996). A cross cultural comparison of ethical perspectives and decision approaches of business students: United States of America versus New Zealand. Journal of Business Ethics, 15, 537–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, Y., & Sparks, J. R. (2012). Predictors, consequence, and measurement of ethical judgments: Review and meta-analysis. Journal of Business Research, 65, 84–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 879–891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Assessing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Randall, D. M., & Fernandes, M. F. (1991). The social desirability response bias in ethics research. Journal of Business Ethics, 10, 805–817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2002). Identifying ethical issues: The roles of issue characteristics, individual differences, and context in managerial sensemaking. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts International. 63, 1907.

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Moral awareness and ethical predispositions: Investigating the role of individual differences in the recognition of moral issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 233–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, S. J. (2008). Moral attentiveness: Who pays attention to the moral aspects of life? Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1027–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robin, D., & Babin, L. (1997). Making sense of the research on gender and ethics in business: A critical analysis and extension. Business Ethics Quarterly, 7, 61–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruegger, D., & King, E. W. (1992). A study of the effect of age and gender upon student business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 11, 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Azzna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25 (pp. 1–65). Boston: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen, W., Kiger, T. B., Davies, S. E., Rasch, R. L., Simon, K. M., & Ones, D. S. (2011). Samples in applied psychology: Over a decade of research in review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(5), 1055–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, M. W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 1–83). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & Miller, J. G. (1985). The social construction of the person: How is it possible? In K. J. Kenneth & K. E. Davis (Eds.), The social construction of the person (pp. 41–69). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. S. (1996). The role of moral intensity and fairness perception in judgments of ethicality: A comparison of managerial professionals and the general public. Journal of Business Ethics, 15, 459–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M. S., & Singer, A. E. (1997). Observer judgments about moral agents’ ethical decisions: The role of scope of justice and moral intensity. Journal of Business Ethics, 16, 473–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singhapakdi, A. (1999). Perceived importance of ethics and ethical decisions in marketing. Journal of Business Research, 45, 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singhapakdi, A., Salyachivin, S., Virakul, B., & Veerayangkur, V. (2000). Some important factors underlying ethical decision making of managers in Thailand. Journal of Business Ethics, 27(3), 271–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., & Franke, G. R. (1999). Antecedents, consequences, and mediating effects of perceived moral intensity and personal moral philosophies. The Journal of Academy of Marketing Science, 27(1), 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., & Kraft, K. L. (1996). Moral intensity and ethical decision-making of marketing professionals. Journal of Business Research, 36, 245–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. L., & Oakley, E. F. (1997). Gender related differences in ethical and social values of business student: Implications for management. Journal of Business Ethics, 16(1), 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffensen, M. S., Joag-Dev, C., & Anderson, R. C. (1979). A Cross-cultural perspective on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 10–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, L. D., Marrs, S., Millar, M. G., & Cole, E. (1984). Processing time and the recall of inconsistent and consistent behaviors of individuals and groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 253–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social information processing’. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The ontario symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 89–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hippel, W., Jonides, J., Hilton, J. L., & Narayan, S. (1993). Inhibitory effect of schematic processing on perceptual encoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 921–935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, J. (1992). Scenarios in business ethics research: Review, critical assessment, and recommendations. Business Ethics Quarterly, 2, 137–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, J., & Glyptis, S. (2000). Measuring the impact of a business ethics course and community service experience on students’ values and opinions. Teaching Business Ethics, 4(4), 341–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, H. (1980). A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator and a direct test for heteroskedasticity. Econometrica, 48, 817–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Resampling and distribution of the product methods for testing indirect effects in complex models. Structural Equation Modeling, 15, 23–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wooldridge, J. M. (2000). Introductory econometrics: A modern approach. Cincinnati: South-Western College Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in its social context. Psychological Review, 93, 322–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S, Jr, Srull, T. K., Gordon, S. E., & Hartwick, J. (1982). Effects of processing objectives on the recall of prose material. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 674–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kurt Wurthmann.

Ethics declarations

Compliance with ethical standards

This study was determined to be EXEMPT FROM FEDERAL REGULATIONS by the University Institutional Review Board. All persons gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Vignette: A COMPANY CAR

Manipulation: (duty-based violation/no harm)

“A manager in your area, Terry, drives a company car. Company policy states that corporate cars are to be inspected every 3000 miles without exception. Terry routinely has her car inspected about every 5000 miles—she says that she ‘just doesn’t want to be bothered that often.’” (Reynolds 2006, p. 243)

There has never been a mechanical problem with Terry’s company car. (new text developed for the present study)

Vignette: COMPANY DRUG TESTING

Manipulation: (rights-based violation/no harm)

Your friend Janice works for a company whose management implemented a mandatory drug testing policy last year to combat the potentially serious negative effects of illegal drug use. It was recently revealed that, in addition to identifying traces of illegal drugs in employees’ bodies, the drug test also identifies traces of legal drugs that can easily be matched to particular medical conditions of employees. The employee’s union argues that management is collecting complete medical histories of all employees, and that the potential exists that this information may be used against employees during the course of their careers. However, there have been no formal complaints of adverse career actions based on employee medical conditions identified by the drug tests and the mandatory drug testing policy remains in effect. The company remains highly profitable. (new text developed for the present study)

Appendix 2

Vignette: A HEALTH PRODUCTS COMPANY (version 1)

Manipulation: (duty-based violation/no harm)

“One of your most important customers, a medical clinic, called yesterday. They had ordered a product 10 days ago (products are normally delivered within 7–10 days), but it had not arrived. Quickly, you traced the order to the shipping office. You asked the shipping clerk about the order and she said, “I shipped it two days ago!” As you left the shipping clerk’s office, you glanced at her desk and saw her shipping receipts. You could clearly see that the order had shipped this morning. You called the clinic back to let them know the product was on its way. As you talked with them you learned that the delay of the product had not affected any patients in any way.” (Reynolds 2006, p. 242)

Vignette: A HEALTH PRODUCTS COMPANY (version 2)

Manipulation: (rights-based violation/no harm)

“One of your most important customers, a medical clinic, called yesterday. They had ordered a product 10 days ago (products are normally delivered within 7–10 days), but it had not arrived. Quickly, you traced the order to the shipping office. You asked the shipping clerk about the order and she said,” (Reynolds 2006, p. 242)

“I tried to ship the order last week, but the shipping company sent a driver in his late sixties to make the pick-up and I believe senior citizens are often unreliable, so I held the order until 2 days ago when the shipping company sent a younger driver for the pick- up. The order still shipped on time.” (new text developed for the present study)

“As you left the shipping clerk’s office, you glanced at her desk and saw her shipping receipts. You could clearly see that the order had shipped 2 days ago. You called the clinic back to let them know the product was on its way. As you talked with them you learned that the delay of the product had not affected any patients in any way.” (Reynolds 2006, p. 242)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wurthmann, K. Implicit Theories and Issue Characteristics as Determinants of Moral Awareness and Intentions. J Bus Ethics 142, 93–116 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2714-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-015-2714-1

Keywords

Navigation