Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Heredity × environment or developmental interactions?Dennis J. Delprato - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):297-298.
    This commentary acknowledges the importance of Davey's biocognitive approach to the uneven distribution of fears on the basis of its contribution to a human model for understanding fear. An integrated heredity-environment and developmental transactional approach based on field/system theory is recommended in place of the mechanistic heredity × environment interactionism that Davey uses to explain behavioral ontogeny.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions.Juan D. Delius - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-685.
  • The purpose of experiments: Ecological validity versus comparing hypotheses.Robyn M. Dawes - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):20-20.
    As illustrated by research Koehler himself cites (Dawes et al. 1993), the purpose of experiments is to choose between contrasting explanations of past observations – rather than to seek statistical generalizations about the prevalence of effects. True external validity results not from sampling various problems that are representative of “real world” decision making, but from reproducing an effect in the laboratory with minimal contamination (including from real world factors).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What defines a legitimate issue for Skinnerian psychology: Philosophy or technology?Hank Davis - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):137-138.
  • The "Disgusting" Spider: The Role of Disease and Illness in the Perpetuation of Fear of Spiders.Graham C. L. Davey - 1994 - Society and Animals 2 (1):17-25.
    Recent studies of spider phobia have indicated thatfearof spiders is closely associated with the disease-avoidance response of disgust. It is argued that the disgust-relevant status of the spider resulted from its association with disease and illness in European cultures from the tenth century onward. The development of the association between spiders and illness appears to be linked to the many devastating and inexplicable epidemics that struck Europe from the Middle Ages onwards, when the spider was a suitable displaced target for (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Preparedness and phobias: Specific evolved associations or a generalized expectancy bias?Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):289-297.
    Most phobias are focussed on a small number of fear-inducing stimuli (e.g., snakes, spiders). A review of the evidence supporting biological and cognitive explanations of this uneven distribution of phobias suggests that the readiness with which such stimuli become associated with aversive outcomes arises from biases in the processing of information about threatening stimuli rather than from phylogenetically based associative predispositions or “biological preparedness.” This cognitive bias, consisting of a heightened expectation of aversive outcomes following fear-relevant stimuli, generates and maintains (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda.Hank Davis & Rachelle Pérusse - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):561-579.
  • Numerical competence: From backwater to mainstream of comparative psychology.Hank Davis & Rachelle Pérusse - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):602-615.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Expectancy bias and phobias: Accounting for the uneven distribution of fears and the characteristics of clinical phobias.Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):315-325.
  • Myth, ritual, and the archetypal hypothesis.Eugene G. D'Aquli - 1986 - Zygon 21 (2):141-160.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Apologia pro scriptura sua, or maybe we got it right after all.Eugene G. D'Aquili - 1993 - Zygon 28 (2):251-266.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Visual discrimination pretraining facilitates subsequent visual cue/toxicosis conditioning in rats.Andrew J. Dalrymple & Bennett G. Galef - 1981 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 18 (5):267-270.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Simplicity and the Meaning of Mental Association.Mike Dacey - 2019 - Erkenntnis 84 (6):1207-1228.
    Some thoughts just come to mind together. This is usually thought to happen because they are connected by associations, which the mind follows. Such an explanation assumes that there is a particular kind of simple psychological process responsible. This view has encountered criticism recently. In response, this paper aims to characterize a general understanding of associative simplicity, which might support the distinction between associative processing and alternatives. I argue that there are two kinds of simplicity that are treated as characteristic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Simplicity and the Meaning of Mental Association.Mike Dacey - 2019 - Erkenntnis 84 (6):1207-1228.
    Some thoughts just come to mind together. This is usually thought to happen because they are connected by associations, which the mind follows. Such an explanation assumes that there is a particular kind of simple psychological process responsible. This view has encountered criticism recently. In response, this paper aims to characterize a general understanding of associative simplicity, which might support the distinction between associative processing and alternatives. I argue that there are two kinds of simplicity that are treated as characteristic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Evidence for the innateness of deontic reasoning.Denise Dellarosa Cummins - 1996 - Mind and Language 11 (2):160-90.
    When reasoning about deontic rules (what one may, should, or should not do in a given set of circumstances), reasoners adopt a violation‐detection strategy, a strategy they do not adopt when reasoning about indicative rules (descriptions of purported state of affairs). I argue that this indicative‐deontic distinction constitutes a primitive in the cognitive architecture. To support this claim, I show that this distinction emerges early in development, is observed regardless of the cultural background of the reasoner, and can be selectively (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Dominance hierarchies and the evolution of human reasoning.Denise Dellarosa Cummins - 1996 - Minds and Machines 6 (4):463-480.
    Research from ethology and evolutionary biology indicates the following about the evolution of reasoning capacity. First, solving problems of social competition and cooperation have direct impact on survival rates and reproductive success. Second, the social structure that evolved from this pressure is the dominance hierarchy. Third, primates that live in large groups with complex dominance hierarchies also show greater neocortical development, and concomitantly greater cognitive capacity. These facts suggest that the necessity of reasoning effectively about dominance hierarchies left an indelible (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • The sociopathy of sociobiology.Wim E. Crusio - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):552-552.
    Mealey's evolutionary reasoning is logically flawed. Furthermore, the evidence presented in favor of a genetic contribution to the causation of sociopathy is overinterpreted. Given the potentially large societal impact of sociobiological speculation on the roots of criminality, more-than-usual caution in interpreting data is called for.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Are base rates a natural category of information?Terry Connolly - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):19-20.
    The base rate fallacy is directly dependent on a particular judgment paradigm in which information may be unambiguously designated as either “base rate” or “individuating,” and in which subjects make two-stage sequential judgments. The paradigm may be a poor match for real world settings, and the fallacy may thus be undefined for natural ecologies of judgment.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Prisoner's Dilemma, Chicken, and mixedstrategy evolutionary equilibria.Andrew M. Colman - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):550-551.
    Mealey's interesting interpretation of sociopathy is based on an inappropriate two-person game model. A multiperson, compound game version of Chicken would be more suitable, because a population engaging in random pairwise interactions with that structure would evolve to an equilibrium in which a fixed proportion of strategic choices was exploitative, antisocial, and risky, as required by Mealey's interpretation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Operant conditioning and natural selection.Andrew M. Colman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):684-685.
  • The need for a theory of evidential weight.L. Jonathan Cohen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):18-19.
    There is a familiar risk of antinomy if fromxisEand p(xisH/xisE) =rit is permissible to infer p(xisH) =r, and what Carnap (1950) called “The requirement of total evidence” will not prevent such antinomies satisfactorily. What is needed instead is a properly developed theory of evidential weight.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Status of the rationality assumption in psychology.Marvin S. Cohen - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):332-333.
  • Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated?L. Jonathan Cohen - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):317-370.
    The object of this paper is to show why recent research in the psychology of deductive and probabilistic reasoning does not have.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   477 citations  
  • Are there any a priori constraints on the study of rationality?L. Jonathan Cohen - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):359-370.
  • Light is an effective warning signal in avoidance learning.George A. Cicala & Julian L. Azorlosa - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (1):70-72.
  • Yoked control designs for assessment of contingency.Russell M. Church - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):451.
  • Laboratory mice do not exhibit fear in the presence of a rattlesnake.David Chiszar - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (4):377-378.
  • Human infants are perhaps not so gifted after all.Bernadette Chauvin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):583-583.
  • An ecological approach toward a unified theory of learning.William R. Charlesworth - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):142-143.
  • Why contingencies won't go away.A. Charles Catania & Eliot Shimoff - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):450.
  • Cheaters never prosper, sometimes.H. Lorne Carmichael - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):549-550.
    In the Frank (1988) model, a small increase in the number of cheaters will soon be reversed. It is not clear that this prediction holds for sociopathy. There are also many attractive evolutionary models that do not admit a small, stable proportion of cheaters. Hence, without definitive evidence about the character of early human society, we cannot conclude that sociopathy has an evolutionary origin.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A different view of numerical processes in animals.E. J. Capaldi & Daniel J. Miller - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):582-583.
  • Sociopathy or hyper-masculinity?Anne Campbell - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (3):548-549.
    Definitional slippage threatens to equate secondary sociopathy with mere criminality and leaves the status of noncriminal sociopaths ambiguous. Primary sociopathy appears to show more environmental contingency than would be implied by a strong genetic trait approach. A reinterpretation in terms of hypermasculinity and hypofemininity is compatible with the data.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The implications of Koehler's approach for fact finding.Craig R. Callen - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (1):18-18.
    Koehler's work will assist the effort to understand legal fact finding. It leaves two questions somewhat open: (i) the extent to which empirical research can measure correctness of fact-finding, a function that involves the resolution of normative questions and (ii) the standards judges should use in the absence of the research he advocates.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Subitizing and rhythm in serial numerical investigations with animals.Richard A. Burns - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):581-582.
  • Ethology and operant psychology.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):683-684.
  • Concurrent counting of two and three events in a serial anticipation paradigm.Richard A. Burns & Rebecca E. Sanders - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (6):479-481.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  • Fear conditioning and extinction as a function of escape from black to white vs. escape from white to black.Gary E. Brown, Rod Guthrie & Paul Blaes - 1975 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (6):450-452.
  • Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Adaptive Content Biases in Learning about Animals across the Life Course.James Broesch, H. Clark Barrett & Joseph Henrich - 2014 - Human Nature 25 (2):181-199.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Reconsidering linguistic nativism from an interdisciplinary, emergentist perspective.Michael Breyl - 2023 - Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 5 (2):162-193.
    For decades, interdisciplinary research efforts have accumulated insights that diminish the significance of the classic nature versus nurture dichotomy, instead calling for a nuanced, multifactorial approach to ontogeny. Similarly, the role of genes in both phylogeny and ontogeny, once seen as rather deterministic, is now conceptualized as highly dependent on environmental factors, including behavior. Linguistic theories have, in principle, made an effort to incorporate these changing views. However, the central claim of the given paper is that this apparent compliance with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Validation of behavioural equations: Can neurobiology help?C. M. Bradshaw - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):136-137.
  • Protocounting as a last resort.Richard F. Braaten - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):581-581.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A theory in need of defense.Marc N. Branch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):678-679.
  • Kanting processes in the chimpanzee: What really counts?Sarah T. Boysen - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):580-580.
  • Incubation and the relevance of functional CS exposure.T. D. Borkovec - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):168-168.
  • The response problem.Robert C. Bolles - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):135-136.
  • The nonextinction of fear: operation bootstrap.Robert C. Bolles - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (2):167-168.
  • The motivation and/or reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):677-678.
  • The bathwater and everything.Robert C. Bolles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):449.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark