Switch to: References

Citations of:

Science and Human Behavior

New York: Free Press (1963)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Behavioral definition of pain: Necessary but not sufficient.Joseph H. Atkinson & Edwin F. Kremer - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):54-55.
  • Book reviews. [REVIEW]Robert L. Arrington - 1998 - Philosophia 26 (3-4):545-549.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some dubious neurological assumptions of radical behaviourism.Jerrold L. Aronson - 1976 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 6 (1):49–60.
  • Stress and arousal in pain perception.Mortimer H. Appley - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):301-302.
  • Contiguity, contingency, and causation.R. J. Andrew - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):447.
  • Skinner's circus.Stuart A. Altmann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):678-679.
  • Brain mechanisms in classical conditioning.A. Alexieva & N. A. Nicolov - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):137-137.
  • What is this cognition that is supposed to be embodied?Ken Aizawa - 2015 - Philosophical Psychology 28 (6):755-775.
    Many cognitive scientists have recently championed the thesis that cognition is embodied. In principle, explicating this thesis should be relatively simple. There are, essentially, only two concepts involved: cognition and embodiment. After articulating what will here be meant by ‘embodiment’, this paper will draw attention to cases in which some advocates of embodied cognition apparently do not mean by ‘cognition’ what has typically been meant by ‘cognition’. Some advocates apparently mean to use ‘cognition’ not as a term for one, among (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Matching is the integrating framework.George Ainslie - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):679-680.
  • How do people choose between local and global bookkeeping?George Ainslie - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):574-575.
    The matching law accounts for both addictive behavior and the usefulness of a person's evaluating choices in overall categories. To explain why overall bookkeeping, once learned, does not easily win out over local bookkeeping, another implication of matching is needed: intertemporal bargaining. The role of melioration, though probably important for new addiction is separate.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grasping the Impalpable: The Role of Endogenous Reward in Choices, Including Process Addictions.George Ainslie - 2013 - Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 56 (5):446 - 469.
    ABSTRACT The list of proposed addictions has recently grown to include television, videogames, shopping, day trading, kleptomania, and use of the Internet. These activities share with a more established entry, gambling, the property that they require no delivery of a biological stimulus that might be thought to unlock a hardwired brain process. I propose a framework for analyzing that class of incentives that do not depend on the prediction of physically privileged environmental events: people have a great capacity to coin (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Behavior is what can be reinforced.George Ainslie - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):53-54.
  • A defense of ignorance.Jonathan E. Adler - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):621.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motivational systems: fear or defense? pain or recuperation?David B. Adams - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (2):301-301.
  • Defense motivational system: Issues of emotion, reinforcement, and neural structure.David Adams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):675-676.
  • Psychologism reconsidered: A re-evaluation of the arguments of Frege and Husserl.John Aach - 1990 - Synthese 85 (2):315 - 338.
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait.Marvin Zuckerman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):413-434.
    A comparative method of studying the biological bases of personality compares human trait dimensions with likely animal models in terms of genetic determination and common biological correlates. The approach is applied to the trait of sensation seeking, which is defined on the human level by a questionnaire, reports of experience, and observations of behavior, and on the animal level by general activity, behavior in novel situations, and certain types of naturalistic behavior in animal colonies. Moderately high genetic determination has been (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • Phylogenic and ontogenic environments.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):701-711.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neuropsychology vis-à-vis Skinner's behaviouristic psychology.Gerhard D. Wassermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):700-701.
  • Each behavior is a product of heredity and experience.Douglas Wahlsten - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):699-700.
  • Reinforcement is the problem, not the solution: Variation and selection of behavior.J. E. R. Staddon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):697-699.
  • Skinner's practical metaphysic may be impractical.S. N. Salthe - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):696-697.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is evolution of behavior operant conditioning writ large?Anatol Rapoport - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):696-696.
  • Nature and nurture revisited.H. C. Plotkin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):695-696.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hereditary ≠ innate.Robert Plomin & Denise Daniels - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):694-695.
  • B. F. Skinner and the flaws of sociobiology.Anthony J. Perzigian - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):693-694.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Molar concepts and mentalistic theories: A moral perspective.Stephen Kaplan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):692-693.
  • The use of evolutionary analogies and the rejection of state variables by B. F. Skinner.Alejandro Kacelnik & Alasdair Houston - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):691-692.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavior in the light of identified neurons.Graham Hoyle - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):690-691.
  • The structure versus the provenance of behavior.Jerry A. Hogan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):690-690.
  • Ethology ignored Skinner to its detriment.Jack P. Hailman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):689-690.
  • Lingering Haeckelian influences and certain other inadequacies of the operant viewpoint for phylogeny and ontogeny.Gilbert Gottlieb - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):688-689.
  • B. F. Skinner versus Dr. Pangloss.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):687-688.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner's blind eye.H. J. Eysenck - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):686-687.
  • Difficulties with phylogenetic and ontogenetic concepts.Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-686.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consequence contingencies and provenance partitions.Juan D. Delius - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):685-685.
  • Operant conditioning and natural selection.Andrew M. Colman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):684-685.
  • Ethology and operant psychology.Gordon M. Burghardt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):683-684.
  • Cost–benefit models and the evolution of behavior.Jerram L. Brown - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):682-682.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new experimental analysis of behavior – one for all behavior.D. Caroline Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard & Kevin J. Flannelly - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):681-682.
  • Of false dichotomies and larger frames.Jerome H. Barkow - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):680-681.
  • Contingencies of selection, reinforcement, and survival.David P. Barash - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):680-680.
  • Ontogenetic or phylogenetic – another afterpain of the fallacious Cartesian dichotomy.Gerard P. Baerends - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):679-680.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):669-677.
    Responses are strengthened by consequences having to do with the survival of individuals and species. With respect to the provenance of behavior, we know more about ontogenic than phylogenic contingencies. The contingencies responsible for unlearned behavior acted long ago. This remoteness affects our scientific methods, both experimental and conceptual. Until we have identified he variables responsible for an event, we tend to invent causes. Explanatory entities such as “instincts,” “drives,” and “traits” still survive. Unable to show how organisms can behave (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Theoretical contingencies.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):541-546.
  • Behavioral and statistical theorists and their disciples.Leroy Wolins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):540-541.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Psychology: Toward the mathematical inner man.James T. Townsend - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):539-540.
  • Behavior, theories, and the inner.Ernest Sosa - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):537-539.