Switch to: References

Citations of:

The Functions of the Brain

Mind 2 (5):92-98 (1877)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Further advantages of abandoning the locality assumption in face recognition.Jules Davidoff & Bernard Renault - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):68-68.
  • The making of a memory mechanism.Carl F. Craver - 2003 - Journal of the History of Biology 36 (1):153-95.
    Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) is a kind of synaptic plasticity that many contemporary neuroscientists believe is a component in mechanisms of memory. This essay describes the discovery of LTP and the development of the LTP research program. The story begins in the 1950's with the discovery of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus (a medial temporal lobe structure now associated with memory), and it ends in 1973 with the publication of three papers sketching the future course of the LTP research program. The (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Temporal representation in the control of movement.Daniel M. Corcos - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-206.
    Theories of the representation of specific kinetic and spatiotem-poral features of movement range from the explicit assertion that temporal aspects of movement are not represented to the idea that they are represented and that they have neurophysiological correlates. Jeannerod's thesis is that mental and visual images have common mechanisms and that there is a link between the image to move and the mechanisms involved with movement. The target article takes the position that certain parameters are coded in motor representations but (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Right and left as symbols.M. C. Corballis - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):636-637.
  • The creative brain: Symmetry breaking in motor imagery.José L. Contreras-Vidal, Jean P. Banquet, Jany Brebion & Mark J. Smith - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):204-205.
  • Modularity, abstractness and the interactive brain.James M. Clark - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):67-68.
  • Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.
  • Inner structure of cortical columns.Kao Liang Chow - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):500-501.
  • Hughlings Jackson and the “doctrine of concomitance”: mind-brain theorising between metaphysics and the clinic.M. Chirimuuta - 2017 - History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 39 (3):26.
    John Hughlings Jackson is a major figure at the origins of neurology and neuroscience in Britain. Alongside his contributions to clinical medicine, he left a large corpus of writing on localisation of function in the nervous system and other theoretical topics. In this paper I focus on Jackson’s “doctrine of concomitance”—his parallelist theory of the mind-brain relationship. I argue that the doctrine can be given both an ontological and a causal interpretation, and that the causal aspect of the doctrine is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The diversity of variability.William D. Chapple - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):602-602.
  • Modularity, interaction and connectionist neuropsychology.Nick Chater - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):66-67.
  • Casting one's net too widely?D. P. Carey & A. D. Milner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):65-66.
  • Variations of reflex parameters and their implications for the control of movements.Charles Capaday & Richard B. Stein - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-602.
  • Locality, modularity and numerical cognition.Jamie I. D. Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):63-64.
  • Discarding locality assumptions: Problems and prospects.Ruth Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):64-65.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Regional specialities.Brian Butterworth - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):63-63.
  • Posterior parietal cortex: Unity or independence of functions?Charles M. Butter - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):500-500.
  • Local representations without the locality assumption.A. Mike Burton & Vicki Bruce - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):62-63.
  • On the limitations of imaging imagining.Christopher A. Buneo & Martha Flanders - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-203.
  • Simulating nonlocal systems: Rules of the game.John A. Bullinaria - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):61-62.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reinventing hemisphere differences.John L. Bradshaw - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):635-635.
  • The compass of the parietal “command” system.Edoardo Bisiach - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):499-500.
  • In search of the theoretical basis of motor control.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):626-638.
  • Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):585-599.
  • Looking down, around, and up: Mechanistic explanation in psychology.William Bechtel - 2009 - Philosophical Psychology 22 (5):543-564.
    Accounts of mechanistic explanation have emphasized the importance of looking down—decomposing a mechanism into its parts and operations. Using research on visual processing as an exemplar, I illustrate how productive such research has been. But once multiple components of a mechanism have been identified, researchers also need to figure out how it is organized—they must look around and determine how to recompose the mechanism. Although researchers often begin by trying to recompose the mechanism in terms of sequential operations, they frequently (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  • Multiple Realizability Revisited: Linking Cognitive and Neural States.William Bechtel - 1999 - Philosophy of Science 66 (2):175-207.
    The claim of the multiple realizability of mental states by brain states has been a major feature of the dominant philosophy of mind of the late 20th century. The claim is usually motivated by evidence that mental states are multiply realized, both within humans and between humans and other species. We challenge this contention by focusing on how neuroscientists differentiate brain areas. The fact that they rely centrally on psychological measures in mapping the brain and do so in a comparative (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   196 citations  
  • Do object affordances represent the functionality of an object?Ruzena Bajcsy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-202.
  • Do legs have surplus degrees of freedom?R. McN Alexander - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-600.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Complexity in control of movements.Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):599-600.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.
  • What counts as local?Andrew W. Young - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):88-89.
  • On the hierarchy of “reflexes”.Uwe Windhorst - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):625-626.
  • The mystery-mastery-imagery complex.H. T. A. Whiting & R. P. Ingvaldsen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):228-229.
  • Clinical features of hemi-inattention.Edwin A. Weinstein - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):518-520.
  • The localization/distribution distinction in neuropsychology is related to the isomorphism/multiple meaning distinction in cell electrophysiology.Gerald S. Wasserman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):87-88.
  • Muscles or Movements? Representation in the Nascent Brain Sciences.Zina B. Ward - 2023 - Journal of the History of Biology 56 (1):5-34.
    The idea that the brain is a representational organ has roots in the nineteenth century, when neurologists began drawing conclusions about what the brain represents from clinical and experimental studies. One of the earliest controversies surrounding representation in the brain was the “muscles versus movements” debate, which concerned whether the motor cortex represents complex movements or rather fractional components of movement. Prominent thinkers weighed in on each side: neurologists John Hughlings Jackson and F.M.R. Walshe in favor of complex movements, neurophysiologist (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Potential disparities between imagining and preparing motor skills.Charles B. Walter & Stephan P. Swinnen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):227-228.
  • Imagery needs preparation too.Stefan Vogt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):226-227.
  • The symbolic brain or the invisible hand?René van Hezewijk & Edward H. F. de Haan - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):85-86.
  • Playing Flourens to Fodor's Gall.Tim van Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-84.
  • Prosopagnosia, conscious awareness and the interactive brain.Robert Van Gulick - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):84-85.
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
  • The functional architecture of visual attention may still be modular.Carlo Umiltà - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):82-83.
  • Beyond anatomical specificity.M. T. Turvey - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):624-625.
  • Clinical disorders of ocular motor control.B. Todd Troost - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):518-518.
  • Attention, motivation, and emotion: Entia non sunt multiplicanda praeter necessitatem.Roger K. Thomas - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):517-518.
  • Action propre and action commune: The localization of cerebral function.Judith P. Swazey - 1970 - Journal of the History of Biology 3 (2):213-234.
  • Separability of reference frame distinctions from motor and visual images.Gary W. Strong - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):224-225.
  • A command or association funtion for the posterior parietal cortex?J. Stein - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):516-517.
  • The Meaning of “Inhibition” and the Discourse of Order.Roger Smith - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):237-263.
    The ArgumentThe history of psychology, like other human science subjects, should attend to the meaning of words understood as relationships of reference and value within discourse. It should seek to identify and defend a history centered on representations of knowledge. The history of the word “inhibition” in nineteenth-century Europe illustrates the potential of such an approach. This word was significant in mediating between physiological and psychological knowledge and between technical and everyday understanding. Further, this word indicated the presence of a (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark