Switch to: References

Citations of:

Ethnographic atlas

[Pittsburgh]: University of Pittsburgh Press (1967)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Experience, culture, and reality: The significance of Fisher information for understanding the relationship between alternative states of consciousness and the structures of reality.Charles D. Laughlin & C. Jason Throop - 2003 - International Journal of Transpersonal Studies 22 (1):7-26.
    The majority of the world’s cultures encourage or require members to enter alternative states of consciousness while involved in religious rituals. The question is, why? This paper suggests an explanation for the culturally prescribed ASC from the view of Fisher information. It argues from the position, first put forward by Emile Durkheim in his magnum opus, The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life, that all religions are grounded in reality. It suggests that many of the structural elements of cultural cosmologies (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Sexual rivalry in human inbreeding or adaptive cooperation?Chet S. Lancaster - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):109-110.
  • A case for less selfing and more outbreeding in reviewing the literature.Michael E. Lamb & Eric L. Charnov - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):109-109.
  • Postmarital Residence and Bilateral Kin Associations among Hunter-Gatherers.Karen L. Kramer & Russell D. Greaves - 2011 - Human Nature 22 (1-2):41-63.
    Dispersal of individuals from their natal communities at sexual maturity is an important determinant of kin association. In this paper we compare postmarital residence patterns among Pumé foragers of Venezuela to investigate the prevalence of sex-biased vs. bilateral residence. This study complements cross-cultural overviews by examining postmarital kin association in relation to individual, longitudinal data on residence within a forager society. Based on cultural norms, the Pumé have been characterized as matrilocal. Analysis of Pumé marriages over a 25-year period finds (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • From genes to culture: The missing links.Joseph K. Kovach - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):15-17.
  • Time to integrate sociobiology and social psychology.Douglas T. Kenrick & Richard C. Keefe - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):24-26.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • God’s punishment and public goods.Dominic D. P. Johnson - 2005 - Human Nature 16 (4):410-446.
    Cooperation towards public goods relies on credible threats of punishment to deter cheats. However, punishing is costly, so it remains unclear who incurred the costs of enforcement in our evolutionary past. Theoretical work suggests that human cooperation may be promoted if people believe in supernatural punishment for moral transgressions. This theory is supported by new work in cognitive psychology and by anecdotal ethnographic evidence, but formal quantitative tests remain to be done. Using data from 186 societies around the globe, I (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Concepts of development in the mathematics of cultural change.Timothy D. Johnston - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):14-15.
  • The psychological foundations of the hero-ogre story.Ian Jobling - 2001 - Human Nature 12 (3):247-272.
    Stories in which a hero defeats a semi-human ogre occur much more frequently in unrelated cultures than chance alone can account for. This claim is supported by a discussion of folk-tales from 20 cultures and an examination of the folk-tales from a random sample of 44 cultures. The tendency to tell these stories must, therefore, have its source in the innate human nature discussed by evolutionary psychologists. This essay argues that these stories reinforce innate positive biases in the perception of (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Intergenerational and Sibling Conflict Under Patrilocality.Ting Ji, Jing-Jing Xu & Ruth Mace - 2014 - Human Nature 25 (1):66-79.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Incest avoidance: shall we drop the genetic leash?William Irons - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):108-109.
  • Mating preferences surveys: Ethnographic follow-up would be a good next step.William Irons - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):24-24.
  • Darwin and the puzzle of primogeniture.Sarah Blaffer Hrdy & Debra S. Judge - 1993 - Human Nature 4 (1):1-45.
    A historical survey of the inheritance practices of farming families in North America and elsewhere indicates that resource allocations among children differed through time and space with regard to sex bias and equality. Tensions between provisioning all children and maintaining a productive economic entity (the farm) were resolved in various ways, depending on population pressures, the family’s relative resource level, and the number and sex of children.Against a backdrop of generalized son preference, parents responded to ecological circumstances by investing in (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Cultivating male allies.Bonnie Lori Hooks & Penny Anthon Green - 1993 - Human Nature 4 (1):81-107.
    Females make large investments in their children and compete among themselves to establish and maintain privileged relationships with male allies who demonstrate both an ability and a willingness to provide fitness-enhancing advantages. Various “strategies” and their more numerous, associated “tactics” are utilized in the competition. Alleged strategies include using sexuality, producing offspring, assisting the male in his own intrasexual contests, and harassing female competitors. The strategies in question are documented in multiple primate species, including humans living in various times and (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Too many P's in the pod.John Hartung - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):23-23.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Early Humans’ Egalitarian Politics.Marc Harvey - 2014 - Human Nature 25 (3):299-327.
  • A too simple view of population genetics.Daniel L. Hartl - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):13-14.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “culturgen”: Science or science fiction?C. R. Hallpike - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):12-13.
  • A Bioeconomic Approach to Marriage and the Sexual Division of Labor.Michael Gurven, Jeffrey Winking, Hillard Kaplan, Christopher von Rueden & Lisa McAllister - 2009 - Human Nature 20 (2):151-183.
    Children may be viewed as public goods whereby both parents receive equal genetic benefits yet one parent often invests more heavily than the other. We introduce a microeconomic framework for understanding household investment decisions to address questions concerning conflicts of interest over types and amount of work effort among married men and women. Although gains and costs of marriage may not be spread equally among marriage partners, marriage is still a favorable, efficient outcome under a wide range of conditions. This (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Genes for general intellect rather than particular culture.Howard E. Gruber - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (1):11-12.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intersocietal variation in the mate preferences of males and females.Norval D. Glenn - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):21-23.
  • Missing link in mate preference studies: Reproduction.Brian A. Gladue - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):21-21.
  • Ethnographic data in the age of big data: How to compare and combine.Kristoffer Lind Glavind & Andreas Bjerre-Nielsen - 2022 - Big Data and Society 9 (1).
    Big data enables researchers to closely follow the behavior of large groups of individuals by using high-frequency digital traces. However, these digital traces often lack context, and it is not always clear what is measured. In contrast, data from ethnographic fieldwork follows a limited number of individuals but can provide the context often lacking from big data. Yet, there is an under-explored potential in combining ethnographic data with big data and other digital data sources. This paper presents ways that quantitative (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Darwinism versus neo-Darwinism in the study of human mate preferences.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):20-20.
  • Preculture versus culture?Daniel G. Freedman - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):107-108.
  • From bridewealth to dowry?Laura Fortunato, Clare Holden & Ruth Mace - 2006 - Human Nature 17 (4):355-376.
    Significant amounts of wealth have been exchanged as part of marriage settlements throughout history. Although various models have been proposed for interpreting these practices, their development over time has not been investigated systematically. In this paper we use a Bayesian MCMC phylogenetic comparative approach to reconstruct the evolution of two forms of wealth transfers at marriage, dowry and bridewealth, for 51 Indo-European cultural groups. Results indicate that dowry is more likely to have been the ancestral practice, and that a minimum (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Spouse preference shifts with age.Susan M. Essock - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):19-20.
  • Risk, Uncertainty, and Violence in Eastern Africa.Carol R. Ember, Teferi Abate Adem & Ian Skoggard - 2013 - Human Nature 24 (1):33-58.
    Previous research on warfare in a worldwide sample of societies by Ember and Ember (Journal of Conflict Resolution, 36, 242–262, 1992a) found a strong relationship between resource unpredictability (particularly food scarcity caused by natural disasters) in nonstate, nonpacified societies and overall warfare frequency. Focusing on eastern Africa, a region frequently plagued with subsistence uncertainty as well as violence, this paper explores the relationships between resource problems, including resource unpredictability, chronic scarcity, and warfare frequencies. It also examines whether resource scarcity predicts (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • More an ideologically driven sermon than science – a review of Randy Thornhill and Craig T. Palmer, a natural history of rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion. [REVIEW]Dorothy Einon - 2002 - Biology and Philosophy 17 (3):445-456.
  • Schellenberg’s Hiddenness Argument and its Reversal.Marek Dobrzeniecki & Jacek Wojtysiak - 2022 - Philosophia 50 (4):1687-1705.
    The article discusses a response to Schellenberg’s atheistic ‘hiddenness argument’ that neither objects to its premises nor formulates a new inductive argument in favour of the existence of God. According to the proposed response, it is sufficient for the task of defending theism to reverse Schellenberg’s reasoning and present a theistic meta-argument that takes as its assumption the fact that there are resistant believers in the world. The paper defends the claim that both arguments have similar persuasive power. However, because (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Aggregates, averages, and behavioral plasticity.Mildred Dickemann - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):18-19.
  • Moral emotions.Ronald de Sousa - 2001 - Ethical Theory and Moral Practice 4 (2):109-126.
    Emotions can be the subject of moral judgments; they can also constitute the basis for moral judgments. The apparent circularity which arises if we accept both of these claims is the central topic of this paper: how can emotions be both judge and party in the moral court? The answer I offer regards all emotions as potentially relevant to ethics, rather than singling out a privileged set of moral emotions. It relies on taking a moderate position both on the question (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Why Do People Become Modern? A Darwinian Explanation.Peter J. Richerson - unknown
    MOST MODERN PEOPLE think it is obvious why people become modern. For them, a more interesting and important puzzle is why some people fail to embrace modern ideas. Why do people in traditional societies often seem unable or unwilling to aspire to a better life for themselves and their children? Why do they fail to see the benefi ts of education, equal rights, democracy, and a rational approach to decisionmaking? What is the glue that makes them adhere to superstition, religion, (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Initial Conditions as Exogenous Factors in Spatial Explanation.Clint Ballinger - 2008 - Dissertation, University of Cambridge
    This dissertation shows how initial conditions play a special role in the explanation of contingent and irregular outcomes, including, in the form of geographic context, the special case of uneven development in the social sciences. The dissertation develops a general theory of this role, recognizes its empirical limitations in the social sciences, and considers how it might be applied to the question of uneven development. The primary purpose of the dissertation is to identify and correct theoretical problems in the study (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The ecology of religious beliefs.Botero Carlos, A. Gardner, Beth Kirby, R. Kathryn, Bulbulia Joseph, Gavin Michael, C. Gray & D. Russell - 2014 - Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (47):16784--16789.
    Although ecological forces are known to shape the expression of sociality across a broad range of biological taxa, their role in shaping human behavior is currently disputed. Both comparative and experimental evidence indicate that beliefs in moralizing high gods promote cooperation among humans, a behavioral attribute known to correlate with environmental harshness in nonhuman animals. Here we combine fine-grained bioclimatic data with the latest statistical tools from ecology and the social sciences to evaluate the potential effects of environmental forces, language (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Open Peer Commentary.Frédéric Bassoa & Olivier Oullierb - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (5).
  • Supernatural punishment and individual social compliance across cultures.Pierrick Bourrat, Quentin Atkinson & Robin Dunbar - 2011 - Religion, Brain and Behavior 1 (2):119-134.
    Cooperation for the public good is vulnerable to exploitation by free-riders because it always pays individuals to exploit the social contract for their own benefit. This problem can be resolved if free-riders are punished, but punishment is itself a public good subject to free-riding. The fear of supernatural punishment hypothesis (FSPH) proposes that belief in supernatural punishment might offer a solution to this problem by deflecting the cost of punishment onto supernatural forces and thereby incentivizing cooperation. FSPH is supported empirically (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations