Switch to: References

Citations of:

The Mismeasure of Man

W.W. Norton and Company (1980)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Taking the intentional stance seriously.Daniel C. Dennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):379-390.
  • The Racialization of White Man's Polygamy.Margaret Denike - 2010 - Hypatia 25 (4):852-874.
    This paper offers a genealogy of anti-polygamy sentiment in North America, elucidating certain racist and nationalist formations that are implicit in the historical valorization and enforcement of heterosexual monogamy. It tracks the white supremacist and heteronormative logic that conditions the widespread disdain toward polygamy, and that renders it fundamentally different from familial configurations that are associated with national identity. Relating political and philosophical doctrines to the archival documentation and insights of contemporary legal and cultural historians of anti-polygamy sentiment, it elucidates (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The 'panglossian paradigm' defended.Daniel C. Dennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):343-90.
    Ethologists and others studying animal behavior in a spirit are in need of a descriptive language and method that are neither anachronistically bound by behaviorist scruples nor prematurely committed to particular Just such an interim descriptive method can be found in intentional system theory. The use of intentional system theory is illustrated with the case of the apparently communicative behavior of vervet monkeys. A way of using the theory to generate data - including usable, testable data - is sketched. The (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   595 citations  
  • Effects of correlation on interactions in the analysis of variance.Victor H. Denenberg - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):129-130.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • O, brave new world: heritability and schizophrenia.Paul J. Dawson - 1997 - Nursing Inquiry 4 (3):202-202.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Monotone interactions: It's even simpler than that.Robyn M. Dawes - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):128-129.
  • Adaptationism was always predictive and needed no defense.Richard Dawkins - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):360-361.
  • Is there a comparative psychology of implicit mathematical knowledge?Hank Davis - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):250-250.
    Geary suggests that implicit mathematical principles exist across human cultures and transcend sex differences. Is such knowledge present in animals as well, and is it sufficient to account for performance in all species, including our own? I attempt to trace the implications of Gearys target article for comparative psychology, questioning the exclusion of “subitizing” in describing human mathematical performance, and asking whether human researchers function as cultural agents with animals, elevating their implicit knowledge to secondary domains of numerical performance.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Constructing gender:: An exploration of afro-american men's conceptualization of manhood.James Earl Davis & Andrea G. Hunter - 1992 - Gender and Society 6 (3):464-479.
    This article explores the meanings of manhood as articulated by Afro-American men. Conceptualization and Q-sort methods are used to examine men's construction of manhood and men's ratings of the importance of selected attributes to being a man. Manhood emerged as a multidimensional construct with four major domains and 15 distinct clusters of ideas. The cluster of attributes rated as most important to being a man paralleled the conceptualization of manhood derived from the open-ended interviews for both professional and nonprofessional men. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Naturalized Female Intellect.Lorraine Daston - 1992 - Science in Context 5 (2):209-235.
    The ArgumentNaturalization confers authority on beliefs, conventions, and claims, but what kind of authority? Because the meaning of nature has a history, so does that of naturalization:naturalization is not the same tactic when marshaled in, say, eighteenth-century France and in late nineteenth-century Britain. Although the authority of nature may be invoked in both cases, the import of that authority depends crucially on whether nature is understood normatively or descriptively, within the framework of the natural laws of jurisprudence or within that (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Science as an international system.Arthur C. Danto - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):359-360.
  • Brain damage from spontaneous but not from induced seizures in animals.Agnete Mouritzen Dam - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):26-26.
  • Estimating heritabilities in quantitative behavior genetics: A station passed.Wim E. Crusio - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):127-128.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How important is detecting interaction?James F. Crow - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):126-127.
  • All sex differences in cognitive ability may be explained by an X-Y homologous gene determining degrees of cerebral asymmetry.T. J. Crow - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):249-250.
    Male superiority in mathematical ability (along with female superiority in verbal fluency) may reflect the operation of an X-Y homologous gene (the right-shift-factor) influencing the relative rates of development of the cerebral hemispheres. Alleles at the locus on the Y chromosome will be selected at a later mean age than alleles on the X, and only by females.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Maintaining underclasses via contrastive judgement: Can inclusive education ever happen?Hilary Cremin & Gary Thomas - 2005 - British Journal of Educational Studies 53 (4):431-446.
    Borrowing from epidemiological and economic analysis, we argue that the central constructs by which children are judged educationally rest in contrastive judgements and that such judgements are based on 'everyday' constructs - not objective descriptors. But because these everyday constructs become seemingly objectified by the procedures and discourses of education, they appear reliable and objective. The insistent process of contrastive judgement based on these everyday constructs has its result in cohorts of children forever being judged unfavourably next to others. A (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Sex differences in age preferences for mates: Primary and secondary predictions from evolutionary theory.Charles Crawford - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):97-98.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Anatomy, metaphysics, and values: The ape brain debate reconsidered. [REVIEW]Christopher Cosans - 1994 - Biology and Philosophy 9 (2):129-165.
    Conventional wisdom teaches that Thomas Huxley discredited Richard Owen in their debate over ape and human brains. This paper reexamines the dispute and uses it as a test case for evaluating the metaphysical realist, internal realist, and social constructivist theories of scientific knowledge. Since Owen worked in the Kantian tradition, his anatomical research illustrates the implications of internal realism for scientific practice. As an avowed Cartesian, Huxley offered a well developed attack on Owen''s position from a metaphysical realist perspective. Adrian (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Straw monkeys.Michael C. Corballis - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):269-270.
  • Right and left as symbols.M. C. Corballis - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):636-637.
  • An even-handed debate? The sexed/gendered controversy over laterality genes in British psychology, 1970s–1990s.Tabea Cornel - 2020 - History of the Human Sciences 33 (5):138-166.
    This article provides insight into the entwinement of the allegedly neutral category of handedness with questions of sex/gender, reproduction, dis/ability, and scientific authority. In the 1860s, Paul Broca suggested that the speech centre sat in the left brain hemisphere in most humans, and that right-handedness stemmed from this asymmetry. One century later, British psychologists Marian Annett and Chris McManus proposed biologically unconfirmed theories of how handedness and brain asymmetry were passed on in families. Their idea to integrate chance into genetic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • It Takes a Village to Trust Science: Towards a (Thoroughly) Social Approach to Public Trust in Science.Gabriele Contessa - 2023 - Erkenntnis 88 (7):2941-2966.
    In this paper, I distinguish three general approaches to public trust in science, which I call the individual approach, the semi-social approach, and the social approach, and critically examine their proposed solutions to what I call the problem of harmful distrust. I argue that, despite their differences, the individual and the semi-social approaches see the solution to the problem of harmful distrust as consisting primarily in trying to persuade individual citizens to trust science and that both approaches face two general (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Science is not always “self-correcting” : fact–value conflation and the study of intelligence.Nathan Cofnas - 2016 - Foundations of Science 21 (3):477-492.
    Some prominent scientists and philosophers have stated openly that moral and political considerations should influence whether we accept or promulgate scientific theories. This widespread view has significantly influenced the development, and public perception, of intelligence research. Theories related to group differences in intelligence are often rejected a priori on explicitly moral grounds. Thus the idea, frequently expressed by commentators on science, that science is “self-correcting”—that hypotheses are simply abandoned when they are undermined by empirical evidence—may not be correct in all (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • On the insensitivity of the ANOVA to interactions: Some suggested simulations.Domenic V. Cicchetti - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):125-126.
  • On viewing the evidence for primate handedness: Some biostatistical considerations.Domenic V. Cicchetti - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):268-269.
  • Dennett' instrumentalism: A frog at the bottom of the mug.Patricia Smith Churchland - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):358-359.
  • Still far too sexy a topic.Susan F. Chipman - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):248-249.
    Geary is highly selective in his use of the literature on gender differences. His assumption of consistent female inferiority in mathematics is not necessarily supported by the facts.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Additivity, interaction, and developmental good sense.David A. Chiszar & Eugene S. Gollin - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):124-125.
  • Talking about race in a scientific context.Frances S. Chew - 2000 - Science and Engineering Ethics 6 (4):485-494.
    There are at least two approaches that assist students in understanding complexity and differing interpretations about human diversity and race. Because differing perspectives emerge from data perceived at different levels, different scales provide a tool for understanding relationships among perspectives and understanding the differential importance of specific factors. Constructivist listening, which assists students in examining their own experiences, feelings and understanding, provides a tool for digesting complex new material and learning emotional literacy. It can be applied to dialogue about race (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Possible brain damage by electroconvulsive therapy: Memory impairment and cultural resistance.Arthur Cherkin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):25-26.
  • Inheritance and the additive genetic model.James M. Cheverud - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):124-124.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Women's neuroethics? Why sex matters for neuroethics.Molly C. Chalfin, Emily R. Murphy & Katrina A. Karkazis - 2008 - American Journal of Bioethics 8 (1):1 – 2.
    The Neuroethics Affinity Group of the American Society for Bioethics and Humanities met for the third time in October 2007 to review progress in the field of neuroethics and consider high-impact priorities for the future. Closely aligned with ASBH's own goals of recruiting junior scholars to bioethics and mentoring them to successful careers, the Neuroethics Affinity Group placed a call for new ideas to be presented at the Group meeting, specifically by junior attendees. One group responded with the idea to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Whiteness of AI.Stephen Cave & Kanta Dihal - 2020 - Philosophy and Technology 33 (4):685-703.
    This paper focuses on the fact that AI is predominantly portrayed as white—in colour, ethnicity, or both. We first illustrate the prevalent Whiteness of real and imagined intelligent machines in four categories: humanoid robots, chatbots and virtual assistants, stock images of AI, and portrayals of AI in film and television. We then offer three interpretations of the Whiteness of AI, drawing on critical race theory, particularly the idea of the White racial frame. First, we examine the extent to which this (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Teachers’ Conceptions About the Genetic Determinism of Human Behaviour: A Survey in 23 Countries.Jérémy Castéra & Pierre Clément - 2014 - Science & Education 23 (2):417-443.
  • Do gender differences in spatial skills mediate gender differences in mathematics among high-ability students?M. Beth Casey - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (2):247-248.
    Based on Geary's theory, intelligence may determine which males utilize innate spatial knowledge to inform their mathematical solutions. This may explain why math gender differences occur mainly with higher abilities. In support, we found that mental rotation ability served as a mediator of gender differences on the math Scholastic Assessment Test for two high-ability samples. Our research suggests, however, that environment and biology interact to influence mental rotation abilities.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Minding Matter/Mattering Mind: Knowledge and the Subject in Nineteenth-Century Psychology.John Carson - 1999 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 30 (3):345-376.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Minding matter/mattering mind: Knowledge and the subject in nineteenth-century psychology.John Carson - 1999 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 30 (3):345-376.
  • Interaction between genotype and environment: Yes, but who truly demonstrates this kind of interaction?Michèle Carlier & Catherine Marchaland - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):123-124.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On evolutionary expectations of symmetry and toolmaking.William H. Calvin - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):267-268.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Differential age preferences: The need to test evolutionary versus alternative conceptualizations.Donn Byrne & Kathryn Kelley - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):96-96.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Methodological heterogeneity and the anachronistic status of ANOVA in psychology.Daniel Bullock - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):122-123.
  • Handedness is a matter of degree.M. P. Bryden & Runa E. Steenhuis - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):266-267.
  • Reaching or manipulation: Left or right?Bernadette Brésard & François Bresson - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):265-266.
  • The May-September algorithm meets the 20th century actuarial table.Gwen J. Broude - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):94-95.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Electroshock therapy and brain damage: The acute organic brain syndrome as treatment.Peter R. Breggin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):24-25.
  • Towards a Feminist Reassessment of Intellectual Virtue.Jane Braaten - 1990 - Hypatia 5 (3):1 - 14.
    This paper presents an argument for reconceptualizing (human) intelligence as intellectual virtue, and makes some proposals as to how we would understand intellectual virtue if feminist values were taken into account. Several abilities are identified which are closely connected to one aim that is common to most feminists: the building of communities in which well-being is possible.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Reinventing hemisphere differences.John L. Bradshaw - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):635-635.
  • J. M. Keynes's position on the general applicability of mathematical, logical and statistical methods in economics and social science.Michael Emmett Brady - 1988 - Synthese 76 (1):1 - 24.
    The author finds no support for the claim that J. M. Keynes had severe reservations, in general, as opposed to particular, concerning the application of mathematical, logical and statistical methods in economics. These misinterpretations rest on the omission of important source material as well as a severe misconstrual ofThe Treatise on Probability (1921).
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • But what about nonprimate asymmetries and nonmanual primate asymmetries?John L. Bradshaw - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):264-265.
  • Age preferences: The crucial studies have yet to be done.Peter Borkenau - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (1):93-94.