Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The risks of rationalising cognitive development.Beatrice de Gelder - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):713-714.
  • Redescribing redescription.Terry Dartnall - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):712-713.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Creativity, combination, and cognition.Terry Dartnall - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):537-537.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Mindset of Cognitive Science.Rick Dale - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (4):e12952.
  • Learning from human tutoring.Michelene T. H. Chi, Stephanie A. Siler, Heisawn Jeong, Takashi Yamauchi & Robert G. Hausmann - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (4):471-533.
    Human one‐to‐one tutoring has been shown to be a very effective form of instruction. Three contrasting hypotheses, a tutor‐centered one, a student‐centered one, and an interactive one could all potentially explain the effectiveness of tutoring. To test these hypotheses, analyses focused not only on the effectiveness of the tutors' moves, but also on the effectiveness of the students' construction on learning, as well as their interaction. The interaction hypothesis is further tested in the second study by manipulating the kind of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Eliciting Self‐Explanations Improves Understanding.Michelene T. H. Chi, Nicholas Leeuw, Mei‐Hung Chiu & Christian Lavancher - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (3):439-477.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Eliciting Self-Explanations Improves Understanding.Michelene T. H. Chi, Nicholas De Leeuw, Mei-Hung Chiu & Christian Lavancher - 1994 - Cognitive Science 18 (3):439-477.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • False beliefs and naive beliefs: They can be good for you.Roberto Casati & Marco Bertamini - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (6):512-513.
    Naive physics beliefs can be systematically mistaken. They provide a useful test-bed because they are common, and also because their existence must rely on some adaptive advantage, within a given context. In the second part of the commentary we also ask questions about when a whole family of misbeliefs should be considered together as a single phenomenon.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):711-712.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Representational redescription and cognitive architectures.Antonella Carassa & Maurizio Tirassa - 1994 - Carassa, Antonella and Tirassa, Maurizio (1994) Representational Redescription and Cognitive Architectures. [Journal (Paginated)] 17 (4):711-712.
    We focus on Karmiloff-Smith's Representational redescription model, arguing that it poses some problems concerning the architecture of a redescribing system. To discuss the topic, we consider the implicit/explicit dichotomy and the relations between natur al language and the language of thought. We argue that the model regards how knowledge is employed rather than how it is represented in the system.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evaluating the inverse reasoning account of object discovery.Christopher D. Carroll & Charles Kemp - 2015 - Cognition 139:130-153.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • What's getting redescribed?Robert L. Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):710-711.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On doing the impossible.Robert L. Campbell - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):535-537.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Analogy programs and creativity.Bruce D. Burns - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):535-535.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is the difference between real creativity and mere novelty?Alan Bundy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):533-534.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Learning to explore the structure of kinematic objects in a virtual environment.Marcus Buckmann, Robert Gaschler, Sebastian Höfer, Dennis Loeben, Peter A. Frensch & Oliver Brock - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lady Lovelace had it right: Computers originate nothing.Selmer Bringsjord - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):532-533.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The theory-ladenness of observation and the theory-ladenness of the rest of the scientific process.William F. Brewer & Bruce L. Lambert - 2001 - Philosophy of Science 68 (3):S176-S186.
    We use evidence from cognitive psychology and the history of science to examine the issue of the theory-ladenness of perceptual observation. This evidence shows that perception is theory-laden, but that it is only strongly theory-laden when the perceptual evidence is ambiguous or degraded, or when it requires a difficult perceptual judgment. We argue that debates about the theory-ladenness issue have focused too narrowly on the issue of perceptual experience, and that a full account of the scientific process requires an examination (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Active inductive inference in children and adults: A constructivist perspective.Neil R. Bramley & Fei Xu - 2023 - Cognition 238 (C):105471.
  • Formalization and Analysis of Reasoning by Assumption.Tibor Bosse, Catholijn M. Jonker & Jan Treur - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (1):147-180.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sticking to the Evidence? A Behavioral and Computational Case Study of Micro‐Theory Change in the Domain of Magnetism.Elizabeth Bonawitz, Tomer D. Ullman, Sophie Bridgers, Alison Gopnik & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (8):e12765.
    Constructing an intuitive theory from data confronts learners with a “chicken‐and‐egg” problem: The laws can only be expressed in terms of the theory's core concepts, but these concepts are only meaningful in terms of the role they play in the theory's laws; how can a learner discover appropriate concepts and laws simultaneously, knowing neither to begin with? We explore how children can solve this chicken‐and‐egg problem in the domain of magnetism, drawing on perspectives from computational modeling and behavioral experiments. We (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Representational redescription: A question of sequence.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):708-708.
  • Précis of The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):519-531.
    What is creativity? One new idea may be creative, whereas another is merely new: What's the difference? And how is creativity possible? These questions about human creativity can be answered, at least in outline, using computational concepts. There are two broad types of creativity, improbabilist and impossibilist. Improbabilist creativity involves novel combinations of familiar ideas. A deeper type involves METCS: the mapping, exploration, and transformation of conceptual spaces. It is impossibilist, in that ideas may be generated which – with respect (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Creativity: A framework for research.Margaret A. Boden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):558-570.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Fodorian guide to Switzerland: Jung and Piaget combined?Péter Bodor & Csaba Pléh - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):709-710.
  • Troubling Anomalies and Exciting Conjectures: A Bipolar Model of Scientific Discovery.Bruno R. Bocanegra - 2017 - Emotion Review 9 (2).
    A model is proposed to explain how emotional and cognitive processes drive epistemic activities within individual scientists. In this account, emotion–cognition interactions produce cyclical phases of accommodative and assimilative epistemic activities, called thought experiments and empirical experiments, respectively. During thought experiments, scientists ruminate over troubling anomalies in order to generate the theoretical ingredients necessary for constructing new conjectures. During empirical experiments, scientists explore exciting conjectures in order to cover the empirical ground necessary to discover new anomalies. Critically, epistemic activities are (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The real problem with constructivism.Paul Bloom & Karen Wynn - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):707-708.
  • The Empirical Attitude, Material Practice and Design Activities.Xornam Apedoe & Michael Ford - 2010 - Science & Education 19 (2):165-186.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Accommodating Surprise in Taxonomic Tasks: The Role of Expertise.Eugenio Alberdi, Derek H. Sleeman & Meg Korpi - 2000 - Cognitive Science 24 (1):53-91.
    This paper reports a psychological study of human categorization that looked at the procedures used by expert scientists when dealing with puzzling items. Five professional botanists were asked to specify a category from a set of positive and negative instances. The target category in the study was defined by a feature that was unusual, hence situations of uncertainty and puzzlement were generated. Subjects were asked to think aloud while solving the tasks, and their verbal reports were analyzed. A number of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Can artificial intelligence explain age changes in literary creativity?Carolyn Adams-Price - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):532-532.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • 創造的問題解決における多様性と評価 洞察研究からの知見.鈴木 宏昭 - 2004 - Transactions of the Japanese Society for Artificial Intelligence 19:145-153.
    The dynamic constraint relaxation theory predicts crucial roles of the initial diversity and evaluation in creative problem-solving. We reported the experimental evidence supporting these predictions, using an insight problem. The experiments showed that the degrees of making different types of trials and the appropriate evaluation were closely related to individual differences in insight problem-solving, and that evaluation became more appropriate by making the problem-solving goal explicit. The review of the research in related fields showed that these experimental findings were in (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • AlphaGo, Locked Strategies, and Eco-Cognitive Openness.Lorenzo Magnani - 2019 - Philosophies 4 (1):8.
    Locked and unlocked strategies are at the center of this article, as ways of shedding new light on the cognitive aspects of deep learning machines. The character and the role of these cognitive strategies, which are occurring both in humans and in computational machines, is indeed strictly related to the generation of cognitive outputs, which range from weak to strong level of knowledge creativity. I maintain that these differences lead to important consequences when we analyze computational AI programs, such as (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Machine discoverers: Transforming the spaces they explore.Jan M. Zytkow - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):557-558.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From the decline of development to the ascent of consciousness.Philip David Zelazo - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):731-732.
  • The creative mind versus the creative computer.Robert W. Weisberg - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):555-557.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Impact of Goal Specificity on Strategy Use and the Acquisition of Problem Structure.Regina Vollmeyer, Bruce D. Burns & Keith J. Holyoak - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (1):75-100.
    Theories of skill acquisition have made radically different predictions about the role of general problem‐solving methods in acquiring rules that promote effective transfer to new problems. Under one view, methods that focus on reaching specific goals, such as means‐ends analysis, are assumed to provide the basis for efficient knowledge compilation (Anderson, 1987), whereas under an alternative view such methods are believed to disrupt rule induction (Sweller, 1988). We suggest that the role of general methods in learning varies with both the (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Is there an implicit level of representation?Annie Vinter & Pierre Perruchet - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):730-731.
  • The empirical detection of creativity.Han L. J. van der Maas & Peter C. M. Molenaar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):555-555.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • “What if…”: The Use of Conceptual Simulations in Scientific Reasoning.Susan Bell Trickett & J. Gregory Trafton - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (5):843-875.
    The term conceptual simulation refers to a type of everyday reasoning strategy commonly called “what if” reasoning. It has been suggested in a number of contexts that this type of reasoning plays an important role in scientific discovery; however, little direct evidence exists to support this claim. This article proposes that conceptual simulation is likely to be used in situations of informational uncertainty, and may be used to help scientists resolve that uncertainty. We conducted two studies to investigate the relationship (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Creativity: Myths? Mechanisms.Michel Treisman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):554-555.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ulcers and bacteria I: discovery and acceptance.Paul Thagard - 1998 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 29 (1):107-136.
    In 1983, Dr. J. Robin Warren and Dr. Barry Marshall reported finding a new kind of bacteria in the stomachs of people with gastritis. Warren and Marshall were soon led to the hypothesis that peptic ulcers are generally caused, not by excess acidity or stress, but by a bacterial infection. Initially, this hypothesis was viewed as preposterous, and it is still somewhat controversial. In 1994, however, a U. S. National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Panel concluded that infection appears to (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Ulcers and bacteria I: discovery and acceptance.Paul Thagard - 1998 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 29 (1):107-136.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Software-realized inquiry support for cultivatig a disciplinary stance.Iris Tabak & Brian Reiser - 2008 - Pragmatics and Cognition 16 (2):307-355.
    What role can technology play in cultivating a disciplinary stance — raising questions, planning investigations, interpreting data and constructing explanations in a way that reflects disciplinary values and principles? How can overt and tacit expert scientific knowledge be captured, represented and used to design software that enables novices to assume a disciplinary stance in their investigations? We present The Galapagos Finches software designed to foster a biological and evolutionary stance. Our approach, Discipline-Specific Strategic Support , translates the main variable types, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Inferring causal networks from observations and interventions.Mark Steyvers, Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers & Ben Blum - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (3):453-489.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   99 citations  
  • Can computers be creative, or even disappointed?Robert J. Sternberg - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):553-554.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Hypothesis testing: Strategy selection for generalising versus limiting hypotheses.Barbara A. Spellman - 1999 - Thinking and Reasoning 5 (1):67 – 92.
    Humans appear to follow normative rules of inductive reasoning in "premise diversity tasks" that is, they know that dissimilar rather than similar evidence is better for generalising hypotheses. In three experiments, we use a "hypothesis limitation task" to compare a related inductive reasoning skill knowing how to limit hypotheses by using a negative test strategy. Participants are told that one category member has some property (e.g. Dogs have a merocrine gland) and are asked what evidence they would test to ensure (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Modal knowledge and transmodularity.Leslie Smith - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):729-730.
  • Individual differences, developmental changes, and social context.Dean Keith Simonton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):552-553.
  • The challenge of representational redescription.Thomas R. Shultz - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (4):728-729.
  • How Do Creative Experts Practice New Skills? Exploratory Practice in Breakdancers.Daichi Shimizu & Takeshi Okada - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (7):2364-2396.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations