Results for 'regular and irregular past tense'

996 found
Order:
  1.  16
    Interpreting dissociations between regular and irregular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Paul de Mornay Davies & Diane Swick - 2008 - Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 8 (2):178–194.
    Neuropsychological dissociations between regular and irregular English past-tense morphology have been reported using a lexical decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets. We present N400 event-related potential data from healthy participants using the same design. Both regular and irregular past-tense forms primed corresponding present-tense forms, but with a longer duration for irregular verbs. Phonological control conditions suggested that differences in formal overlap between prime and (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  21
    The role of Broca's area in regular past-tense morphology.Timothy Justus, Jary Larsen, Jennifer Yang, Paul de Mornay Davies, Nina Dronkers & Diane Swick - 2011 - Neuropsychologia 49 (1):1–18.
    It has been suggested that damage to anterior regions of the left hemisphere results in a dissociation in the perception and lexical activation of past-tense forms. Specifically, in a lexical-decision task in which past-tense primes immediately precede present-tense targets, such patients demonstrate significant priming for irregular verbs (spoke–speak), but, unlike control participants, fail to do so for regular verbs (looked–look). Here, this behavioral dissociation was first confirmed in a group of eleven patients with (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  11
    The Past-Tense Debate The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  4. Children's Frequency , Productivity Phonology, in the and English Past Tense : The Role of Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school-aged children (N= 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using on elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero-marking, e.g., sit + sit; vowel-change, e.g., ride -+ rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in (...)
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  51
    Children's Acquisition of the English PastTense: Evidence for a Single‐Route Account From Novel Verb Production Data.Ryan P. Blything, Ben Ambridge & Elena V. M. Lieven - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):621-639.
    This study adjudicates between two opposing accounts of morphological productivity, using English past-tense as its test case. The single-route model posits that both regular and irregular past-tense forms are generated by analogy across stored exemplars in associative memory. In contrast, the dual-route model posits that regular inflection requires use of a formal “add -ed” rule that does not require analogy across regular past-tense forms. Children saw animations of an animal performing (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  3
    The past-tense debate.S. Pinker & M. T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  7.  9
    The past and future of the past tense.Steven Pinker & Michael Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  8.  17
    An event-related potential study of cross-modal morphological and phonological priming.Timothy Justus, Jennifer Yang, Jary Larsen, Paul de Mornay Davies & Diane Swick - 2009 - Journal of Neurolinguistics 22 (6):584–604.
    The current work investigated whether differences in phonological overlap between the past- and present-tense forms of regular and irregular verbs can account for the graded neurophysiological effects of verb regularity observed in past-tense priming designs. Event-related potentials were recorded from 16 healthy participants who performed a lexical-decision task in which past-tense primes immediately preceded present-tense targets. To minimize intra-modal phonological priming effects, cross-modal presentation between auditory primes and visual targets was employed, (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9. The Past and Future of the Past Tense Debate.Steven Pinker & Michael T. Ullman - 2002 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 (11):456-463.
    What is the interaction between storage and computation in language processing? What is the psychological status of grammatical rules? What are the relative strengths of connectionist and symbolic models of cognition? How are the components of language implemented in the brain? The English past tense has served as an arena for debates on these issues. We defend the theory that irregular past-tense forms are stored in the lexicon, a division of declarative memory, whereas regular (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  33
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.Kim Plunkett & Patrick Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  11.  13
    A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.V. Merlin, M. Tataru, F. Valognes, K. Plunkett & P. Juola - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):463-490.
    The acquisition of English noun and verb morphology is modeled using a single-system connectionist network. The network is trained to produce the plurals and past tense forms of a large corpus of monosyllabic English nouns and verbs. The developmental trajectory of network performance is analyzed in detail and is shown to mimic a number of important features of the acquisition of English noun and verb morphology in young children. These include an initial error-free period of performance on both (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  12.  21
    Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: a computational/experimental study.Adam Albright & Bruce Hayes - 2003 - Cognition 90 (2):119-161.
    Are morphological patterns learned in the form of rules? Some models deny this, attributing all morphology to analogical mechanisms. The dual mechanism model (Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1998). On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73-193) posits that speakers do internalize rules, but that these rules are few and cover only regular processes; the remaining patterns are attributed to analogy. This article advocates a third approach, which uses multiple stochastic (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  13.  10
    Answering the connectionist challenge: a symbolic model of learning the past tenses of English verbs.C. X. Ling & M. Marinov - 1993 - Cognition 49 (3):235-290.
    Supporters of eliminative connectionism have argued for a pattern association-based explanation of language learning and language processing. They deny that explicit rules and symbolic representations play any role in language processing and cognition in general. Their argument is based to a large extent on two artificial neural network (ANN) models that are claimed to be able to learn the past tenses of English verbs (Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986, Parallel distributed processing, Vol. 2, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; MacWhinney & Leinbach, (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  14.  5
    Overtensing and the effect of regularity.Joseph Paul Stemberger - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (6):737-766.
    Regularly inflected forms often behave differently in language production than irregular forms. These differences are often used to argue that irregular forms are listed in the lexicon but regular forms are produced by rule. Using an experimental speech production task with adults, it is shown that overtensing errors, where a tensed verb is used in place of an infinitive, predominantly involve irregular forms, but that the differences may be due to phonological confounds, not to regularity per (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  15. English past tense inflection: regular vs. irregular or easy vs. hard.G. Westermann, V. Kovic & N. Ruh - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 739--744.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  26
    A complex system approach to language evolution.Francesca Colaiori & Francesca Tria - 2020 - Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 2 (2):118-126.
    Regularities in natural language systems, despite their cognitive advantages in terms of storage and learnability, often coexist with exceptions, raising the question of whether and why irregularities survive. We offer a complex system perspective on this issue, focusing on the irregular past tense forms in English. Two separate processes affect the overall regularity: new verbs constantly entering the vocabulary in the regular form at low frequency, and transitions in both directions occurring in a narrow frequency range. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  8
    The acquisition of the English past tense in children and multilayered connectionist networks.Gary F. Marcus - 1995 - Cognition 56 (3):271-279.
    The apparent very close similarity between the learning of the past tense by Adam and the Plunkett and Marchman model is exaggerated by several misleading comparisons--including arbitrary, unexplained changes in how graphs were plotted. The model's development differs from Adam's in three important ways: Children show a U-shaped sequence of development which does not depend on abrupt changes in input; U-shaped development in the simulation occurs only after an abrupt change in training regimen. Children overregularize vowel-change verbs more (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  18.  15
    When Children's Production Deviates From Observed Input: Modeling the Variable Production of the English Past Tense.Libby Barak, Zara Harmon, Naomi H. Feldman, Jan Edwards & Patrick Shafto - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (8):e13328.
    As children gradually master grammatical rules, they often go through a period of producing form‐meaning associations that were not observed in the input. For example, 2‐ to 3‐year‐old English‐learning children use the bare form of verbs in settings that require obligatory past tense meaning while already starting to produce the grammatical –ed inflection. While many studies have focused on overgeneralization errors, fewer studies have attempted to explain the root of this earlier stage of rule acquisition. In this work, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  10
    Detecting Evolutionary Forces in Language Change.Mitchell Newberry, Ahern G., A. Christopher, Robin Clark & Joshua B. Plotkin - 2017 - Nature Publishing Group 551 (7679):223–226.
    Both language and genes evolve by transmission over generations with opportunity for differential replication of forms. The understanding that gene frequencies change at random by genetic drift, even in the absence of natural selection, was a seminal advance in evolutionary biology. Stochastic drift must also occur in language as a result of randomness in how linguistic forms are copied between speakers. Here we quantify the strength of selection relative to stochastic drift in language evolution. We use time series derived from (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20. Predicting irregular past tenses.Charles X. Ling - 1994 - In Ashwin Ram & Kurt Eiselt (eds.), Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society: August 13 to 16, 1994, Georgia Institute of Technology. Erlbaum. pp. 577--582.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  2
    Against Regular and Irregular Characterizations of Mechanisms.Lane DesAutels - 2011 - Philosophy of Science 78 (5):914-925.
    This article addresses the question of whether we should conceive of mechanisms as productive of change in a regular way. I argue that, if mechanisms are characterized as fully regular, on the one hand, then not enough processes will count as mechanisms for them to be interesting or useful. If no appeal to regularity is made at all in their characterization, on the other hand, then mechanisms can no longer be useful for grounding prediction and supporting intervention strategies. (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  22.  12
    Spatial deictic tense and evidentials in Korean.Kyung-Sook Chung - 2007 - Natural Language Semantics 15 (3):187-219.
    This paper focuses on the Korean suffix -te, which has been variously analyzed as a marker of tense, aspect, tense–aspect, mood, mood–tense, or evidentiality. I argue against all of these approaches and propose instead that -te is a spatial deictic past tense, which triggers an evidential environment. It refers to a certain past time when the speaker either observed an event or some evidence of the event within his (her) perceptual field. Thus, the denotation (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  23.  16
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler, Andreas Woest & Gary F. Marcus - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  24.  1
    Meaning, Regular and Irregular.J. F. Staal - 1968 - Foundations of Language 4 (2):182-184.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  11
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler & Andreas Woest - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  26. The nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection.Steven Pinker & Joseph Shimron - unknown
    Most evidence for the role of regular inflection as a default operation comes from languages that confound the morphological properties of regular and irregular forms with their phonological characteristics. For instance, regular plurals tend to faithfully preserve the base’s phonology, whereas irregular nouns tend to alter it. The distinction between regular and irregular inflection may thus be an epiphenomenon of phonological faithfulness. In Hebrew noun inflection, however, morphological regularity and phonological faithfulness can be (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  27. The irregular verbs.Steven Pinker - unknown
    The irregulars are defiantly quirky. Thousands of verbs monotonously take the -ed suffix for their past tense forms, but ring mutates to rang, not ringed, catch becomes caught, hit doesn't do anything, and go is replaced by an entirely different word, went (a usurping of the old past tense of to wend, which itself once followed the pattern we see in send-sent and bend-bent). No wonder irregular verbs are banned in "rationally designed" languages like Esperanto (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  11
    Dissociation between regular and irregular in connectionist architectures: Two processes, but still no special linguistic rules.Marco Zorzi & Gabriella Vigliocco - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1045-1046.
    Dual-mechanism models of language maintain a distinction between a lexicon and a computational system of linguistic rules. In his target article, Clahsen provides support for such a distinction, presenting evidence from German inflections. He argues for a structured lexicon, going beyond the strict lexicon versus rules dichotomy. We agree with the author in assuming a dual mechanism; however, we argue that a next step must be taken, going beyond the notion of the computational system as specific rules applying to a (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  2
    Transfer following regular and irregular sequences of events in a guessing situation.Lawrence S. Meyers, Erik Driessen & Joseph Halpern - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (2):182.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  6
    On the processing of regular and irregular forms of verbs and nouns: evidence from neuropsychology.Michele Miozzo - 2003 - Cognition 87 (2):101-127.
  31.  1
    A note on the regular and irregular modal systems of Lewis.Bolesław Sobociński - 1962 - Notre Dame Journal of Formal Logic 3 (2):109-113.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  32.  12
    Innateness, autonomy, universality, and the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology.David Kemmerer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-641.
    Müller's goal of bringing neuroscience to bear on controversies in linguistics is laudable. However, some of his specific proposals about innateness and autonomy are misguided. Recent studies on the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology indicate that these two linguistic processes are subserved by anatomically and physiologically distinct neural subsystems, whose functional organization is likely to be under direct genetic control rather than assembled by strictly epigenetic factors.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  36
    Past Tense and Past Times in Subjunctive Conditionals.John Mackay - 2017 - Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 98 (S1):520-535.
    Some theories of conditionals maintain that the difference between indicative and subjunctive conditionals involves the standard temporal interpretation of past tense. I provide an argument against such theories. The argument is based on the claim that these views do not correctly predict the correspondence between tense marking and temporal interpretation in certain conditionals.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  2
    Changes in functional connectivity within the fronto-temporal brain network induced by regular and irregular Russian verb production.Maxim Kireev, Natalia Slioussar, Alexander D. Korotkov, Tatiana V. Chernigovskaya & Svyatoslav V. Medvedev - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  35.  3
    Do morphophonological rules impact both regular and irregular verb inflection? Evidence from acquired morphological impairment.Rimikis Stacey & Buchwald Adam - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  10
    Memory, Expression, and PastTense Self‐Knowledge.William Child - 2007 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 73 (1):54-76.
    How should we understand our capacity to remember our past intentional states? And what can we leam from Wittgenstein's treatment of this topic? Three questions are considered. First, what is the relation between our past attitudes and our present beliefs about them? Realism about past attitudes is defended. Second, how should we understand Wittgenstein's view that self‐ascriptions of past attitudes are a kind of “response” and that the “language‐game” of reporting past attitudes is “the primary (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  37.  13
    Simple Past Tense Markers in Turkish and Some American Indian Languages in Terms of Evidentiality.Demi̇rci̇ Kerim - 2010 - Journal of Turkish Studies 5:281-293.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  24
    Regular versus irregular inflection: A question of levels.Alessandro Laudanna - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1029-1030.
    When referring to the organization of the mental lexicon, the distinction between combinatorial rules and lexical listing for regularly versus irregularly inflected words should be further developed to account for subregular morphological processes. Moreover, the distinction may be more or less appropriate depending on the lexical component under consideration, and it is subject to interplay with other factors that are relevant in determining the representational structure of the lexical system.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39. Past tense, logic and pedagogy.L. Tasmowskideryck - 1985 - Revue Internationale de Philosophie 39 (155):375-387.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  13
    On the cross-linguistic validity of a dual-mechanism model.Margherita Orsolini - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1033-1035.
    Recent studies of Italian past definite and past participle forms show that human performance with regular and irregular inflections is not dissociated as Clahsen's model would predict. Some performance profiles, accounted for by dual-mechanism models in terms of an underlying symbol-manipulating combinatorial procedure, are generated in Italian by the higher learnability and generalizability of phonologically regular morphological processes.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  5
    The story of two connectives: Korean tunci ‘or’ and kena ‘or’.Minju Kim - 2021 - Discourse Studies 23 (4):497-518.
    Using 129 natural conversations and 185 episodes of television drama conversations as well as the theoretical frameworks of usage-based theory and grammaticalization, I investigate two forms of ‘or’ in Korean, tunci and kena. Generally believed to be largely interchangeable, these two forms’ actual usages have never been compared. I demonstrate that the two are selectively used in conversation, and propose that three types of factor influence the selection. The first factors are genre and setting. In formal settings and formal descriptive (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  12
    Errors of Omission in English‐Speaking Children's Production of Plurals and the Past Tense: The Effects of Frequency, Phonology, and Competition.Danielle E. Matthews & Anna L. Theakston - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (6):1027-1052.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  43.  1
    Universal and Past-Tense Prescriptions: A Reply to Mr. Ibberson.R. M. Hake - 1979 - Analysis 39 (4):161 - 165.
    Properly universal prescriptions (necessary in analysis of value-judgments) entail past-tense imperatives. does the unusability of the latter rule out the former? no, because there are many usable rules which entail past-tense imperatives. else we could not point to past breaches when teaching the rule, which remains the same throughout the teaching process, or punish for past breaches of the same rule which is still in force. similar problem about imperatives in other than the second (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  44.  20
    Bolesław Sobociński. A note on the regular and irregular modal systems of Lewis. Notre Dame journal of formal logic, vol. 3 , pp. 109–113. [REVIEW]G. F. Schumm - 1972 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 37 (1):181-182.
  45.  2
    The Search for Regularity in Irregularity: Defectiveness and its Implications for our Knowledge of Words.Marianne Mithun - 2010 - In Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us. pp. 125.
    The longstanding issue in morphological theory has been the status of inflected forms in the memory. In general, the irregular forms of words are assumed to be learned, stored, and retrieved for use. While the contention on the storage of irregular forms seemed to be clear and cohesive, the views on the nature of storage of regular words vary. For some, all inflected forms are stored while some contend that storage is not homogenous, wherein the frequently-used forms (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  3
    The English past tense: Analogy redux.Steve Chandler - 2010 - Cognitive Linguistics 21 (3):371-417.
    The debate over how best to characterize inflectional morphology has been couched largely in terms of the “dual-mechanism” approach described in Pinker (Words and rules: the ingredients of language, Basic Books, 1999) versus “single-mechanism” connectionist approaches derived from Rumelhart and McClelland (On learning past tenses of English verbs, MIT, 1986). There are, however, other single-mechanism approaches. The exemplar-based or analogical models of Daelemans et al. (TimBL: Tilburg Memory-Based Learner, version 4.3 reference guide, ILK, 2002) and Skousen (Analogical modeling of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  8
    Universal and past-tense prescriptions: a reply to Mr. Ibberson.R. M. Hake - 1979 - Analysis 39 (4):161-165.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  17
    Memory, expression, and past-tense self-knowledge.William Child - 2006 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 73 (1):54–76.
    How should we understand our capacity to remember our past intentional states? And what can we learn from Wittgenstein's treatment of this topic? Three questions are considered. First, what is the relation between our past attitudes and our present beliefs about them? Realism about past attitudes is defended. Second, how should we understand Wittgenstein's view that self-ascriptions of past attitudes are a kind of "response" and that the "language-game" of reporting past attitudes is "the primary (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49.  10
    Quasiregularity and Its Discontents: The Legacy of the Past Tense Debate.Mark S. Seidenberg & David C. Plaut - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (6):1190-1228.
    Rumelhart and McClelland's chapter about learning the past tense created a degree of controversy extraordinary even in the adversarial culture of modern science. It also stimulated a vast amount of research that advanced the understanding of the past tense, inflectional morphology in English and other languages, the nature of linguistic representations, relations between language and other phenomena such as reading and object recognition, the properties of artificial neural networks, and other topics. We examine the impact of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  50.  5
    A neuroconstructivist model of past tense development and processing.Gert Westermann & Nicolas Ruh - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (3):649-667.
1 — 50 / 996