Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Emotions – inferences from hypothetical hypothalamic circuits?Magda B. Arnold - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):423-423.
  • Assessing internal affairs.Hymie Anisman & Robert M. Zacharko - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):422-423.
  • Defense motivational system: Issues of emotion, reinforcement, and neural structure.David Adams - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):675-676.
  • The defense motivation system: A theory of avoidance behavior.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):661-675.
    A motivational system approach to avoidance behavior is presented. According to this approach, a motivational state increases the probability of relevant response patterns and establishes the appropriate or “ideal” consummatory stimuli as positive reinforcers. In the case of feeding motivation, for example, hungry rats are likely to explore and gnaw, and to learn to persist in activities correlated with the reception of consummatory stimuli produced by ingestion of palatable substances. In the case of defense motivation, fearful rats are likely to (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior.Philippe Soubrié - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):319-335.
    Animal research suggests that central serotonergic neurons are involved in behavioral suppression, particularly anxiety-related inhibition. The hypothesis linking decreased serotonin transmission to reduced anxiety as the mechanism in the anxiolytic activity of benzodiazepines conflicts with most clinical observations. Serotonin antagonists show no marked capacity to alleviate anxiety. On the other hand, clinical signs of reduced serotonergic transmission (low 5-HIAA levels in the cerebrospinal fluid) are frequently associated with aggressiveness, suicide attempts, and increased anxiety. The target article attempts to reconcile such (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Serotonin, impulsivity, and emotionality.Marvin Zuckerman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):348-349.
  • Can arousal be pleasurable?Marvin Zuckerman - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):449-449.
  • Avoidance is in the head, not the genes.Everett J. Wyers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-685.
  • Serotonin depletion and inhibition: Running the rat race without any brakes?David Wirtshafter & Karen E. Asin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):347-348.
  • Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account.Amanda C. De C. Williams - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (4):439-455.
    This paper proposes that human expression of pain in the presence or absence of caregivers, and the detection of pain by observers, arises from evolved propensities. The function of pain is to demand attention and prioritise escape, recovery, and healing; where others can help achieve these goals, effective communication of pain is required. Evidence is reviewed of a distinct and specific facial expression of pain from infancy to old age, consistent across stimuli, and recognizable as pain by observers. Voluntary control (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Is serotonin related to inhibition or generation and control of motor activity?C. H. Vanderwolf - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):346-347.
  • Is attentional prioritization on a location where pain is expected modality-specific or multisensory?Charlotte Vanden Bulcke, Geert Crombez, Wouter Durnez & Stefaan Van Damme - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 36:246-255.
  • Does introspection have a role in brain-behavior research?C. H. Vanderwolf & M. A. Goodale - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):448-448.
  • Controlling a neuron bomb.Luigi Valzelli - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):345-346.
  • Introspection and science: The problem of standardizing emotional nomenclature.Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):447-448.
  • Pain is pain and fear is fear.Holger Ursin - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):318-320.
  • Advances toward a biological theory of aversive learning: Flirtation or commitment?Dallas Treit & Marcia L. Spetch - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):684-685.
  • The rat as hedonist – A systems approach.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):446-447.
  • A cognitive-incentive view.Frederick M. Toates - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):683-684.
  • Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder as a Disturbance of Security Motivation.Henry Szechtman & Erik Woody - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (1):111-127.
  • Softening the wires of human emotion.Michael Stocker - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):445-446.
  • A lonesome Français in serotonin country.Philippe Soubrié - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):349-363.
  • Strategies for studying brain–behavior relationships.Paul R. Solomon - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):344-345.
  • Emotional cookbooks.Robert C. Solomon - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):444-445.
  • Modulating function of central serotonin neurons.E. N. Sokolov - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):344-344.
  • On the nature of specific hard-wired brain circuits.Allan Siegel - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-444.
  • The involvement of serotonergic mechanisms in anxiety and impulsivity in humans.Daisy Schalling - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):343-344.
  • On the complexity of emotion.Joseph R. Royce - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):443-443.
  • Serotonin, simians, and social setting.Michael J. Raleigh - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):342-343.
  • Only four command systems for all emotions?Robert Plutchik - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):442-443.
  • Is there a role for serotonin in anxiety?Sharon Pellow - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):341-342.
  • Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions.Jaak Panksepp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):407-422.
  • Given the chance, the normal brain can casually avoid what it would otherwise intensely fear.Jaak Panksepp & Larry Normansell - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):682-683.
  • An overdue burial for the serotonin theory of anxiety.Jaak Panksepp & James F. Cox - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):340-341.
  • Archaeology of mind.Jaak Panksepp - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):449-467.
  • Reinforcement of avoidance behavior.Arlo K. Myers - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):681-682.
  • Psychobiology needs cognitive psychology.Adam Morton - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):441-442.
  • The propositional nature of human associative learning.Chris J. Mitchell, Jan De Houwer & Peter F. Lovibond - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2):183-198.
    The past 50 years have seen an accumulation of evidence suggesting that associative learning depends on high-level cognitive processes that give rise to propositional knowledge. Yet, many learning theorists maintain a belief in a learning mechanism in which links between mental representations are formed automatically. We characterize and highlight the differences between the propositional and link approaches, and review the relevant empirical evidence. We conclude that learning is the consequence of propositional reasoning processes that cooperate with the unconscious processes involved (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • Avoidance behavior: Assumptions, theory, and metatheory.Fred A. Masterson & Mary Crawford - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):685-696.
  • Conditioning as compensation?N. J. Mackintosh - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (2):318-318.
  • Concerning the alleged four basic emotions.William Lyons - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):440-441.
  • Introspection and cultural knowledge systems.Catherine Lutz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):439-440.
  • Expecting shock.A. W. Logue - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):680-681.
  • Psychobiology without psychosocial significance.Richard S. Lazarus - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):438-439.
  • Generality and specifics in psychobiological theory of emotions.Eric Klinger & Ernest D. Kemble - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):437-438.
  • Panic, separation anxiety, and endorphins.Donald F. Klein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):436-437.
  • Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness.Brian Key - 2015 - Biology and Philosophy 30 (2):149-165.
    Phenomenal consciousness or the subjective experience of feeling sensory stimuli is fundamental to human existence. Because of the ubiquity of their subjective experiences, humans seem to readily accept the anthropomorphic extension of these mental states to other animals. Humans will typically extrapolate feelings of pain to animals if they respond physiologically and behaviourally to noxious stimuli. The alternative view that fish instead respond to noxious stimuli reflexly and with a limited behavioural repertoire is defended within the context of our current (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Parting's sweet sorrow: A pain pathway for the social sentiments?Leonard D. Katz - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):435-436.
  • Serotonin and aggression: What kind of relationship?Pierre Karli - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):340-340.
  • A two-tiered theory of emotions: Affect and feeling.Julian Jaynes - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):434-435.