Results for 'total stimulus information'

987 found
Order:
  1.  8
    Interaction between total stimulus information and specific stimulus information in visual recognition.J. R. Newbrough - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (3):297.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  28
    Stimulus information and contextual information as determinants of tachistoscopic recognition of words.Endel Tulving & Cecille Gold - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (4):319.
  3.  57
    Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time.Ray Hyman - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 (3):188.
  4. Field of view.Thomas Natsoulas - 1998 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 19 (4):415-436.
    Two concepts of field of view are spelled out, the ordinary concept defined by the dictionary and the technical concept devised by Gibson and put to work in his ecological account of visual perceiving. The dictionaryís concept refers to an area of the environment taken from a particular viewpoint; from this viewpoint, there are some objects visible throughout the geographical area constituting the corresponding field of view. The technical concept refers to the total large solid angle of light that (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  21
    Effects of stimulus information reduction on search time of retarded adolescents and normal children.Herman H. Spitz - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):482.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  15
    Information processing behavior: The role of irrelevant stimulus information.Robert E. Morin, Bert Forrin & Wayne Archer - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (1):89.
  7.  14
    Reaction time as a function of stimulus information and age.George J. Suci, Melvin D. Davidoff & Walter W. Surwillo - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (4):242.
  8.  15
    Reduction of redundant stimulus information in short-term memory.Robert E. Morin, Dorothy S. Konick & Kenneth L. Hoving - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 2 (1):29-30.
  9.  11
    Search-discrimination time for missing stimulus information.Aley Thomas & Charles M. Solley - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):501.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  21
    Conceptual distortions of hand structure are robust to changes in stimulus information.Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Luigi Tamè & Matthew R. Longo - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:107-116.
    Hands are commonly held up as an exemplar of well-known, familiar objects. However, conceptual knowledge of the hand has been found to show highly stereotyped distortions. Specifically, people judge their knuckles as farther forward in the hand than they actually are. The cause of this distal bias remains unclear. In Experiment 1, we tested whether both visual and tactile information contribute to the distortion. Participants judged the location of their knuckles by pointing to the location on their palm directly (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  54
    The child's trigger experience: Degree-0 learnability.David Lightfoot - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):321-334.
    According to a “selective” (as opposed to “instructive”) model of human language capacity, people come to know more than they experience. The discrepancy between experience and eventual capacity (the “poverty of the stimulus”) is bridged by genetically provided information. Hence any hypothesis about the linguistic genotype (or “Universal Grammar,” UG) has consequences for what experience is needed and what form people's mature capacities (or “grammars”) will take. This BBS target article discusses the “trigger experience,” that is, the experience (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   93 citations  
  12.  16
    Shared and Unshared Feature Extraction in Major Depression During Music Listening Using Constrained Tensor Factorization.Xiulin Wang, Wenya Liu, Xiaoyu Wang, Zhen Mu, Jing Xu, Yi Chang, Qing Zhang, Jianlin Wu & Fengyu Cong - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Ongoing electroencephalography signals are recorded as a mixture of stimulus-elicited EEG, spontaneous EEG and noises, which poses a huge challenge to current data analyzing techniques, especially when different groups of participants are expected to have common or highly correlated brain activities and some individual dynamics. In this study, we proposed a data-driven shared and unshared feature extraction framework based on nonnegative and coupled tensor factorization, which aims to conduct group-level analysis for the EEG signals from major depression disorder patients (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  43
    Consciousness and cognition may be mediated by multiple independent coherent ensembles.E. Roy John, Paul Easton & Robert Isenhart - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (1):3-39.
    Short-term or working memory provides temporary storage of information in the brain after an experience and is associated with conscious awareness. Neurons sensitive to the multiple stimulus attributes comprising an experience are distributed within many brain regions. Such distributed cell assemblies, activated by an event, are the most plausible system to represent the WM of that event. Studies with a variety of imaging technologies have implicated widespread brain regions in the mediation of WM for different categories of (...). Each kind of WM may thus be expected to involve many brain regions rather than a local, uniquely dedicated set of cells. Neurons in a distributed “cell assembly” may be self-selected by their temporally coherent activations. The process by which this fragmented representation of the recent past is reassembled to accomplish essentially automatic and reliable recognition of a recurrent event constitutes an important problem. One plausible mechanism to achieve the identification of past with previous events would require that the representational system mediating WM must coexist in spatial extent and somehow overlap in temporal activation with cell ensembles registering input from subsequent events. The detection of such a postulated mechanism required an experimental approach which would focus upon spatial patterns of coherent activation while information about different events was stored in WM and retrieved, rather than focusing upon the temporal sequences of activation in localized regions of interest. For this purpose, the familiar delayed matching from sample task was modified. A series of information-free flashes, or “noncontingent probes,” was presented before an initial series of visual information items, the Priming Sample, which were to be held in WM during a Delay Period. A second series of visual information items were then presented, the Matching Sample. The task required detection of any item in the second series which had beenabsentfrom the initial series. Thirty such trials with a particular category of visual information constituted a single task. Several DMS tasks with this standardized design, but with different categories of visual information, were presented within each test session. The information categories included letters of the alphabet, single digit numbers, or faces from a school yearbook. Event-related potentials , were computed from 21 standardized electrode placements, separately for information-free probes and for information items in each interval of the trials within a task. Because each electrode is particularly sensitive to coherent activation of neurons in the immediately underlying brain regions, topographic maps were constructed and interpolated across the surface of the scalp. The momentary fluctuations of the resulting voltage “landscapes” throughout the task were then subjected to quantitative analysis. Distinctive landscapes sometimes persisted for prolonged periods, implying sustained engagement of very large populations of neurons. “Difference landscapes” were constructed by subtraction of topographic maps evoked by noncontingent probes during the Delay Period from maps of probe ERPs before the presentation of the initial information in the Priming Sample. Such probe difference landscapes displayed recurrent high similarity to momentary landscapes elicited during subsequent presentation of the information items in the Matching Sample. It seemed as if the distributed cell assembly continuously engaged by mediation of WM of the diverse attributes of the initial stimuli was being dynamically compared to the ensembles engaged by registration of the subsequent stimuli. Spatial Principal Component Analysis was applied to the sequences of momentary voltage landscapes observed throughout trials of each task. This method sought a small number of spatial patterns with which these large sets of inhomogeneous spatial distributions of voltage could be reconstructed. This is the spatial analog of the reconstruction of local ERPs by temporal principal components, as often described previously. Five Spatial Principal Components were found which accounted for about 90% of the total variance of voltage across the surface of the scalp throughout every task. Theloadings,or distinguishing topographic features, of these SPCs, were highly similar during every cognitive task for every subject. However,factor scores,or relative average contribution to the overall voltage distributions, of the different SPCs varied substantially among subjects between the tasks and momentarily within successive intervals of each task. These five SPCs may reflect coherent activation of huge distributed ensembles of neurons which comprise independent but interacting functional brain subsystems. These subsystems may correspond to basic resources available to individuals for allocation to mediate conscious evaluation of information during cognitive activity, providing a filter to bind together fractionated representations of the past to evaluate the present. (shrink)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  14.  41
    Contingencies and attentional capture: the importance of matching stimulus informativeness in the item-specific proportion congruent task.James R. Schmidt - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
  15.  35
    Automatic and attention-dependent processing of auditory stimulus information.Risto Näätänen - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):261-288.
  16.  21
    Total time and stimulus-response imagery in paired-associate learning.John H. Mueller & Frank L. Slaymaker - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (2):288.
  17.  7
    An Eye-Tracking Study of Sketch Processing: Evidence From Russian.Tatiana E. Petrova, Elena I. Riekhakaynen & Valentina S. Bratash - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    This study investigates the online process of reading and analyzing of sketchnotes (visual notes containing a handwritten text and drawings) on Russian language material. Using the eye-tracking method, we compared the processing of different types of sketchnotes (‘path’ (trajectory), linear, and radial) and the processing of a verbal text. Biographies of Russian writers were used as the material. In a preliminary experiment, we asked 89 college students to read the biographies and to evaluate each text or sketch using five scales (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  16
    Integrating Categorization and Decision‐Making.Rong Zheng, Jerome R. Busemeyer & Robert M. Nosofsky - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (1):e13235.
    Though individual categorization or decision processes have been studied separately in many previous investigations, few studies have investigated how they interact by using a two-stage task of first categorizing and then deciding. To address this issue, we investigated a categorization-decision task in two experiments. In both, participants were shown six faces varying in width, first asked to categorize the faces, and then decide a course of action for each face. Each experiment was designed to include three groups, and for each (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  18
    Spatio-temporal dynamics of face recognition in a flash: itʼs in the eyes.Céline Vinette, Frédéric Gosselin & Philippe G. Schyns - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (2):289-301.
    We adapted the Bubbles procedure [Vis. Res. 41 (2001) 2261] to examine the effective use of information during the first 282 ms of face identification. Ten participants each viewed a total of 5100 faces sub-sampled in space–time. We obtained a clear pattern of effective use of information: the eye on the left side of the image became diagnostic between 47 and 94 ms after the onset of the stimulus; after 94 ms, both eyes were used effectively. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  20.  17
    Spatio-temporal dynamics of face recognition in a flash: itʼs in the eyes.Céline Vinette, Frédéric Gosselin & Philippe G. Schyns - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (2):289-301.
    We adapted the Bubbles procedure [Vis. Res. 41 (2001) 2261] to examine the effective use of information during the first 282 ms of face identification. Ten participants each viewed a total of 5100 faces sub-sampled in space–time. We obtained a clear pattern of effective use of information: the eye on the left side of the image became diagnostic between 47 and 94 ms after the onset of the stimulus; after 94 ms, both eyes were used effectively. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  21.  8
    The impact of different age-friendly smart home interface styles on the interaction behavior of elderly users.Chengmin Zhou, Yawen Qian, Ting Huang, Jake Kaner & Yurong Zhang - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Smart homes create a beneficial environment for the lives of elderly people and enhance the quality of their home lives. This study aims to explore the design of age-friendly interfaces that can meet the emotional needs of self-care elderly people from the perspective of functional realization of the operating interface. Sixteen elderly users aged fifty-five and above were selected as subjects with healthy eyes and no excessive drooping eyelids to obscure them. Four representative age-friendly applications with different interface designs were (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  5
    Stimulus durations and total learning time in paired-associates learning.Calvin F. Nodine - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (5):534.
  23.  14
    How Do Artificial Neural Networks Classify Musical Triads? A Case Study in Eluding Bonini's Paradox.Arturo Perez, Helen L. Ma, Stephanie Zawaduk & Michael R. W. Dawson - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (1):e13233.
    How might artificial neural networks (ANNs) inform cognitive science? Often cognitive scientists use ANNs but do not examine their internal structures. In this paper, we use ANNs to explore how cognition might represent musical properties. We train ANNs to classify musical chords, and we interpret network structure to determine what representations ANNs discover and use. We find connection weights between input units and hidden units can be described using Fourier phase spaces, a representation studied in musical set theory. We find (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  4
    A differentiated look at emotions: association between gaze behaviour during the processing of affective videos and emotional granularity.Jonas Potthoff, Albert Wabnegger & Anne Schienle - 2023 - Cognition and Emotion 37 (8):1349-1356.
    The ability to distinguish between subtle differences among emotions of similar valence is labelled emotion differentiation (ED). Previous research has demonstrated that people high in ED are less likely to use disengagement regulation strategies (i.e. avoidance/distraction) during negative affective states.The present eye-tracking study examined associations between ED and visual attention/avoidance of affective stimuli. A total of 160 participants viewed emotional video clips (positive/ negative), which were concurrently presented with a non-affective distractor image. After each video, participants verbally described their (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  6
    Data leaking scandal, risks, and financial consumption behaviors in online tourism platforms: The role of trust on college students and teachers.Mingfen Feng, Bin Hu, Yaqi Tian & Huabing Wang - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Given the importance of data safety for psychology, the present study investigated the influence of data leaking scandal on campus customers’ financial consumption behaviors at intelligent tourism platforms in China, and explored the roles that individual characteristics play in this process by focusing on a set of participants from colleges. Data were collected through sending out an online questionnaire, where respondents were asked to finish a series of questions about their background information, their trust, future consuming intention, and defensive (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  27
    Information theory and stimulus encoding in free and serial recall: Ordinal position of formal similarity.Douglas L. Nelson - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (3p1):537.
  27.  21
    Information value and stimulus configuring as factors in conditioned reinforcement.David R. Thomas, David L. Berman & George E. Serednesky - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (2p1):181.
  28.  10
    Stimulus-response coding and amount of information as determinants of reaction time.Sidney Hellyer - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):521.
  29.  23
    Information theory and stimulus encoding in paired-associate acquisition: Ordinal position of formal similarity.Douglas L. Nelson & Frank A. Rowe - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 79 (2p1):342.
  30.  47
    Information: In the stimulus or in the context?Giulio Tononi & Gerald M. Edelman - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):698-700.
    The distinction between receptive field and conceptual field is appealing and heuristically useful. Conceptually, it is more satisfactory to distinguish between information from the environment and from the brain. We emphasize here a selectionist view that considers information transmission within the brain as modulated by a stimulus, rather than information transmission from a stimulus as modulated by the context.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31. Time: From the Totality to Quantum Information.Vasil Penchev - 2020 - Philosophy of Science eJournal (Elsevier: SSRN) 13 (24):1-14.
    The paper justifies the following theses: The totality can found time if the latter is axiomatically represented by its “arrow” as a well-ordering. Time can found choice and thus information in turn. Quantum information and its units, the quantum bits, can be interpreted as their generalization as to infinity and underlying the physical world as well as the ultimate substance of the world both subjective and objective. Thus a pathway of interpretation between the totality via time, order, choice, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  20
    Information and incentive value of the reinforcing stimulus in verbal conditioning.Charles D. Spielberger, Ira H. Bernstein & Richard G. Ratliff - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (1):26.
  33.  10
    Stimulus-determined and perceiver-determined aspects of haptic perceptual information processing.Paul J. Locher - 1986 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 24 (3):197-200.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34. Information measures, effective complexity, and total information.Murray Gell-Mann & Seth Lloyd - 1996 - Complexity 2 (1):44-52.
  35.  29
    Effects of stimulus alternation on sequential information processing by retarded and nonretarded subjects.Edward A. Holden - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 101 (2):262.
  36.  22
    On the heterogeneity of stimulus and response elements in the processing of information.William C. Howell - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (3):235.
  37.  54
    Total Information Awareness–Forgotten But Not Gone: Lessons for Neuroethics.Sheri Alpert - 2007 - American Journal of Bioethics 7 (5):24 – 26.
  38.  12
    Information processing of olfactory stimuli by the dog: II. Stimulus control and sampling strategies in simultaneous discrimination learning.R. E. Lubow, Moshe Kahn & Reuven Frommer - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (4):323-326.
  39.  11
    Stimulus size and acuity in information processing.Derek W. Schultz & Charles W. Eriksen - 1978 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12 (6):397-399.
  40.  28
    Choices based on redundant information: An analysis of two-dimensional stimulus control.Sheila Chase & Eric G. Heinemann - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (2):161.
  41.  37
    Secondary reinforcement in rats as a function of information value and reliability of the stimulus.M. David Egger & Neal E. Miller - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (2):97.
  42.  27
    Effect of stimulus condition and reaction time information on spatial stimulus generalization.Charles Y. Nakamura & Jaques W. Kaswan - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (1):67.
  43.  86
    Conditional reasoning under time constraint: Information retrieval and inhibition.Henry Markovits & Hugues Lortie Forgues - 2010 - Thinking and Reasoning 16 (3):221-232.
    A total of 152 students were asked to respond to a series of causal conditional (“If P then Q”) inferences with major premises for which there was variable access to information contradicting the premises. Half the students were given 12.5 s for each inference, the other half were given 8.5 s. The percentage of accepted inferences was significantly lower when the time was shorter for the MP and MT inferences, but no effect was observed for the AC and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  29
    Instructions and stimulus properties in processing relevant-redundant information.Herman Staudenmayer & Roger W. Schvaneveldt - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):175.
  45.  14
    The role of information systems in total quality management.Jaak Jurison - 1994 - Knowledge, Technology & Policy 7 (2):3-16.
    This paper presents a conceptual model for describing the role of information systems in a Total Quality Management (TQM) organization and contrasts it with one for a traditional business firm. The model, based on systems theory, provides a framework for understanding the principles of TQM and their effects on information systems (IS). This paper suggests that TQM introduces changes in the firm’s feedback loop and creates new requirements for the IS function. The TQM system model is also (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46. Two deductions: (1) from the totality to quantum information conservation; (2) from the latter to dark matter and dark energy.Vasil Penchev - 2020 - Information Theory and Research eJournal (Elsevier: SSRN) 1 (28):1-47.
    The paper discusses the origin of dark matter and dark energy from the concepts of time and the totality in the final analysis. Though both seem to be rather philosophical, nonetheless they are postulated axiomatically and interpreted physically, and the corresponding philosophical transcendentalism serves heuristically. The exposition of the article means to outline the “forest for the trees”, however, in an absolutely rigorous mathematical way, which to be explicated in detail in a future paper. The “two deductions” are two successive (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47. The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning.Koch Iring & Hoffmann Joachim - 2000 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26 (4).
  48.  20
    Cognitive aspects of information processing: II. Adjustments to stimulus redundancy.Paul M. Fitts, James R. Peterson & Gerson Wolpe - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (5):423.
  49. Informational realism.Luciano Floridi - manuscript
    What is the ultimate nature of reality? This paper defends an answer in terms of informational realism (IR). It does so in three stages. First, it is shown that, within the debate about structural realism (SR), epistemic (ESR) and ontic (OSR) structural realism are reconcilable by using the methodology of the levels of abstractions. It follows that OSR is defensible from a structuralist-friendly position. Second, it is argued that OSR is also plausible, because not all related objects are logically prior (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  50.  28
    Partial advance information and stimulus dimensionality.Barry H. Kantowitz & Mark S. Sanders - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):412.
1 — 50 / 987