Results for ' inflection'

593 found
Order:
  1. Kenneth L. Miner.English Inflectional Endings & Unordered Rules - 1974 - Foundations of Language 12:339.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2. Lisa Green/Aspectual be–type Constructions and Coercion in African American English Yoad Winter/Distributivity and Dependency Instructions for Authors.Pauline Jacobson, Paycheck Pronouns, Bach-Peters Sentences, Inflectional Head, Thomas Ede Zimmermann, Free Choice Disjunction, Epistemic Possibility, Sigrid Beck & Uli Sauerland - 2000 - Natural Language Semantics 8 (373).
  3. Inflected and uninflected perception of pictures.Bence Nanay - 2010 - In Catharine Abell & Katerina Bantinaki (eds.), Philosophical Perspectives on Depiction. Oxford University Press.
    It has been argued that picture perception is sometimes, but not always, ‘inflected’. Sometimes the picture’s design ‘inflects’, or is ‘recruited’ into the depicted scene. The aim of this paper is to cash out what is meant by these metaphors. Our perceptual state is different when we see an object fact to face or when we see it in a picture. But there is also a further distinction: our perceptual state is very different if we perceive objects in pictures in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  4. Inflected Pictorial Experience: Its Treatment and Significance.Robert Hopkins - 2010 - In Catharine Abell & Katerina Bantinaki (eds.), Philosophical Perspectives on Depiction. Oxford University Press. pp. 151.
    Some (Podro, Lopes) think that sometimes our experience of pictures is ‘inflected’. What we see in these pictures involves, somehow, an awareness of features of their design. I clarify the idea of inflection, arguing that the thought must be that what is seen in the picture is something with properties which themselves need characterising by reference to that picture’s design, conceived as such. I argue that there is at least one case of inflection, so understood. Proponents of (...) have claimed great significance for the phenomenon. But what might that significance be? Inter alia, I consider Lopes’s proposal that inflection solves a central problem in pictorial aesthetics, the ‘puzzle of mimesis’. I argue that the puzzle, and the proposed solution, both turn on aspects of Lopes’s conception of seeing-in. Other accounts of seeing-in can make no sense of either. I further argue that the phenomenon of inflection itself puts pressure on the sort of account Lopes offers. Thus it is hard to offer a view which both holds that inflection occurs and is able to make clear sense of why it matters. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  5.  3
    Inflecting the house: Upside down and ungrounded between walls, windows, mirrors and screens.Tordis Berstrand - 2022 - Technoetic Arts 20 (3):167-182.
    During COVID-19, private living spaces have become settings for activities usually taking place elsewhere. Work, education and leisure activities have moved in, while we have moved out and now frequently project our private interiors onto the screens of others when meeting online. We see ourselves reflected while reflecting each other, and we peek into the lives of strangers while staging our own for the world to see. If such virtual cross-extensions of public and private domains are not completely new, then (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  6. “Inflecting ‘Presence’ and ‘Absence’: On Sharing the Phenomenological Conversation.”.Chad Engelland - 2020 - In Language and Phenomenology. New York: Routledge. pp. 273-295.
    This chapter introduces the difficulty of acquiring phenomenological terms by examining Carnap’s and Derrida’s criticisms of phenomenological speech; their criticisms show that any account of how phenomenological speech is acquired must clarify its distinction from ordinary speech about things while not falling prey to an esoteric separation. The chapter then reviews the way Husserl, Scheler, and Heidegger offer “indication” as the way to distinguish but not separate the one and the other, and it argues that indication, even with the support (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. Inflectional Identity.Asaf Bachrach & Andrew Nevins (eds.) - 2008 - Oxford University Press UK.
    A recurrent issue in linguistic theory and psychology concerns the cognitive status of memorized lists and their internal structure. In morphological theory, the collections of inflected forms of a given noun, verb, or adjective into inflectional paradigms are thought to constitute one such type of list. This book focuses on the question of which elements in a paradigm can stand in a relation of partial or total phonological identity. Leading scholars consider inflectional identity from a variety of theoretical perspectives, with (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  8.  16
    Inflected Language: Toward a Hermeneutics of Nearness: Heidegger, Levinas, Stevens, Celan.Krzysztof Ziarek - 1994 - State University of New York Press.
    Proposes to rethink the ontological and ethical dimensions of language by rereading Heidegger's work and by engaging Levinas' ethics and contemporary poetics.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  9.  31
    Inflection classes, gender, and the principle of contrast.Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy - 1994 - In Stephen Everson (ed.), Language. Cambridge University Press. pp. 737--788.
  10. Inflectional morphology through creolization: richness and predictability of verb inflection in Portuguese and Portuguese-related creoles.Alain Kihm - 2004 - Complexity 8:10.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11.  12
    Production of Inflected Novel Words in Older Adults With and Without Dementia.Alexandre Nikolaev, Eve Higby, JungMoon Hyun, Minna Lehtonen, Sameer Ashaie, Merja Hallikainen, Tuomo Hänninen & Hilkka Soininen - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (8):e12879.
    While cognitive changes in aging and neurodegenerative disease have been widely studied, language changes in these populations are less well understood. Inflecting novel words in a language with complex inflectional paradigms provides a good opportunity to observe how language processes change in normal and abnormal aging. Studies of language acquisition suggest that children inflect novel words based on their phonological similarity to real words they already know. It is unclear whether speakers continue to use the same strategy when encountering novel (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  12.  43
    Finnish noun inflection.Paul Kiparsky - manuscript
    Inflected words in Finnish show a range of interdependent stem and suffix alternations which are conditioned by syllable structure and stress. In a penetrating study, Anttila (1997) shows how the statistical preferences among optional alternants of the Genitive Plural can be derived from free constraint ranking. I propose an analysis which covers the rest of the nominal morphology and spells out the phonological constraints that interact to produce the alternations, and show how it supports a stratal version of OT phonology.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  13. English inflectional endings and unordered rules.C. Householder, Thomas Perry, Catherine Ringen & Gerald Sanders - 1974 - Foundations of Language: International Journal of Language and Philosophy 12:339.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  7
    English inflectional endings and unordered rules.Kenneth L. Miner - 1975 - Foundations of Language 12 (3):339-365.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  47
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler, Andreas Woest & Gary F. Marcus - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  16. Sculpting in time: temporally inflected experience of cinema.Robert Hopkins - 2019 - In Jerome Pelletier & Albert Voltolini (eds.), The Pleasure of Pictures: Pictorial Experience and Aesthetic Appreciation. Routledge. pp. 201-223.
    We engage with all representational pictures by seeing things in them. Seeing-in is a distinctive form of visual experience, one in which we are aware of both the marks, projected lights, or whatever that make up the picture (its Design) and what the picture represents (Scene). Some seeing-in is inflected: what we then see in the picture is a scene the properties of which make essential reference to Design. Since cinema involves moving pictures, it too supports seeing-in. But can that (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  50
    Spanish inflectional morphology in DATR.Antonio Moreno-Sandoval & José Miguel Goñi-Menoyo - 2002 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 11 (1):79-105.
    This paper shows a full description of Spanish inflectional morphology. We have chosen a paradigmatic approach instead of one based on phonological/spelling changes, i.e., the typical two-level model. Such morphological description has been written in the DATR formalism. The result is a network of nodes that makes use of the information inheritance mechanisms – orthogonal node inheritance and default path inheritance – that DATR allows. Some lexical coverage and corpus occurrence figures that support our approach are also given.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  34
    Inflectional classes, defaults, and syncretisms.Andrew Spencer - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1040-1040.
    I argue for an extension of Clahsen's psycholinguistic paradigm to well-known languages with more complex morphological systems. This would help to address conceptual questions such as the nature of defaults and the way in which syncretisms are coded in the brain.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  79
    Why, as responsible for figurativity, seeing-in can only be inflected seeing-in.Alberto Voltolini - 2015 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 14 (3):651-667.
    In this paper, I want to argue for two main and related points. First, I want to defend Richard Wollheim’s well-known thesis that the twofold mental state of seeing-in is the distinctive pictorial experience that marks figurativity. Figurativity is what makes a representation pictorial, a depiction of its subject. Moreover, I want to show that insofar as it is a mark of figurativity, all seeing-in is inflected. That is to say, every mental state of seeing-in is such that the characterisation (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  20.  4
    Rhythmic Bodies: Amplification, Inflection and Transduction in the Dance Performance Techniques of the “Bashment Gal”.Julian Henriques - 2014 - Body and Society 20 (3-4):79-112.
    This article explores the rhythmic body with the example of the embodiment of the ‘bashment gal’ and the role she plays in the dancehall sound system session. It considers rhythm as an energetic patterning process operating both within and between media. Rhythm provides a means of communication and making sense that does not rely on representation or code. There are three elements to performance techniques of the rhythmic body – amplification, inflection and transduction. Amplification for the bashment gal’s performance (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  21.  39
    On Modeling Scope of Inflectional Negation.Alex Lascarides & Emily M. Bender - unknown
    In this paper, we investigate the representation of negated sentences in Minimal Recursion Semantics (Copestake, Flickinger, Pollard, & Sag, 2005). We begin with its treatment in the English Resource Grammar (Flickinger, 2000, 2011), a broad-coverage implemented HPSG (Pollard & Sag, 1994), and argue that it is largely a suitable representation for English, despite possible objections. We then explore whether it is suitable for typologically different languages: namely, those that express sentential negation via inflection on the verb, particularly Turkish and (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22.  29
    Lexical access and inflectional morphology.Alfonso Caramazza, Alessandro Laudanna & Cristina Romani - 1988 - Cognition 28 (3):297-332.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  23.  56
    Rules Versus Statistics: Insights From a Highly Inflected Language.Jelena Mirković, Mark S. Seidenberg & Marc F. Joanisse - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (4):638-681.
    Inflectional morphology has been taken as a paradigmatic example of rule-governed grammatical knowledge (Pinker, 1999). The plausibility of this claim may be related to the fact that it is mainly based on studies of English, which has a very simple inflectional system. We examined the representation of inflectional morphology in Serbian, which encodes number, gender, and case for nouns. Linguists standardly characterize this system as a complex set of rules, with disagreements about their exact form. We present analyses of a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  24.  13
    David Brewster’s and William Herschel’s experiments on inflection that delivered the coup de grâce to Thomas Young’s ether distribution hypothesis.Olivier Morizot - forthcoming - Annals of Science:25.
    In his ‘Theory of Light and Colours’, presented to the Royal Society in November 1801, Thomas Young defended a mechanical explanation of the coloured fringes observed outside of the shadow of an opaque object – the so-called ‘colours by inflection’ – that was based on the hypothesis of an ethereal density gradient surrounding all material bodies. However, two years later, he publicly rejected that hypothesis, without giving much detail of his reasons. Although Geoffrey Cantor has demonstrated the crucial role (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  61
    Regular versus irregular inflection: A question of levels.Alessandro Laudanna - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1029-1030.
    When referring to the organization of the mental lexicon, the distinction between combinatorial rules and lexical listing for regularly versus irregularly inflected words should be further developed to account for subregular morphological processes. Moreover, the distinction may be more or less appropriate depending on the lexical component under consideration, and it is subject to interplay with other factors that are relevant in determining the representational structure of the lexical system.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26. Beyond autopoiesis: Inflections of emergence and politics in the work of francisco Varela.John Protevi - manuscript
    Francisco Varela’s work is a monumental achievement in 20th century biological and biophilosophical thought. After his early collaboration in neo-cybernetics with Humberto Maturana (“autopoiesis”), Varela made fundamental contributions to immunology (“network theory”), Artificial Life (“cellular automata”), cognitive science (“enaction”), philosophy of mind (“neurophenomenology”), brain studies (“the brainweb”), and East- West dialogue (the Mind and Life conferences). In the course of his career, Varela influenced many important collaborators and interlocutors, formed a generation of excellent students, and touched the lives of many (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  27.  16
    Simulating the Acquisition of Verb Inflection in Typically Developing Children and Children With Developmental Language Disorder in English and Spanish.Daniel Freudenthal, Michael Ramscar, Laurence B. Leonard & Julian M. Pine - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (3):e12945.
    Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) have significant deficits in language ability that cannot be attributed to neurological damage, hearing impairment, or intellectual disability. The symptoms displayed by children with DLD differ across languages. In English, DLD is often marked by severe difficulties acquiring verb inflection. Such difficulties are less apparent in languages with rich verb morphology like Spanish and Italian. Here we show how these differential profiles can be understood in terms of an interaction between properties of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28. Building complex events: the case of Sicilian Doubly Inflected Construction.Fabio Del Prete & Giuseppina Todaro - 2020 - Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 38 (1):1-41.
    We examine the Doubly Inflected Construction of Sicilian (DIC; Cardinaletti and Giusti 2001, 2003, Cruschina 2013), in which a motion verb V1 from a restricted set is followed by an event verb V2 and both verbs are inflected for the same person and tense features. The interpretation of DIC involves a complex event which behaves as a single, integrated event by linguistic tests. Based on data drawn from different sources, we argue that DIC is an asymmetrical serial verb construction (Aikhenvald (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  14
    Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals.Harald Clahsen, Monika Rothweiler & Andreas Woest - 1992 - Cognition 45 (3):225-255.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  30.  31
    The processing of inflected forms.Charles Clifton, Anne Cutler, James M. McQueen & Brit van Ooijen - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1018-1019.
    Clahsen proposes two distinct processing routes, for regularly and irregularly inflected forms, respectively, and thus is apparently making a psychological claim. We argue that his position, which embodies a strictly linguistic perspective, does not constitute a psychological processing model.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  47
    Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variables.Joseph Shimron, Iris Berent & Stephen Pinker - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):1-44.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32. Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection.Harald Clahsen - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):991-1013.
    Following much work in linguistic theory, it is hypothesized that the language faculty has a modular structure and consists of two basic components, a lexicon of (structured) entries and a computational system of combinatorial operations to form larger linguistic expressions from lexical entries. This target article provides evidence for the dual nature of the language faculty by describing recent results of a multidisciplinary investigation of German inflection. We have examined: (1) its linguistic representation, focussing on noun plurals and verb (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  33. Krzysztof Ziarek, Inflected Language: Toward a Hermeneutic of Nearness―Heidegger, Levinas, Stevens, Celan.Silvia Benso - 1995 - Philosophy in Review 15 (4):301-302.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34. English past tense inflection: regular vs. irregular or easy vs. hard.G. Westermann, V. Kovic & N. Ruh - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 739--744.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35. German Non-inflectional Constructions as Separate Performatives.Sebastian Bucking & Jennifer Rau - 2013 - In Daniel Gutzmann & Hans-Martin Gärtner (eds.), Beyond Expressives: Explorations in Use-Conditional Meaning. Boston: Brill.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  3
    Chapter VI. Inflections.Blandine Kriegel - 1995 - In The State and the Rule of Law. Princeton University Press. pp. 91-96.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  13
    Whole-word frequency and inflectional paradigm size facilitate Estonian case-inflected noun processing.Kaidi Lõo, Juhani Järvikivi & R. Harald Baayen - 2018 - Cognition 175 (C):20-25.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  38.  74
    Pictorial Aesthetics and Two Kinds of Inflected Seeing-In.Giulia Martina - 2016 - Estetika: The European Journal of Aesthetics 53 (1):74-92.
    Inflected seeing-in is a special experience of the vehicle and subject of a picture, which are experienced as related to each other. Bence Nanay recently defended the idea that inflected picture perception is central to the aesthetic appreciation of pictures. Here I critically discuss his characterization of inflection, and advance a new one, that better accounts for the structure and content of inflected experience in terms of properties of the pictures themselves and also clarifies the distinctive contribution of (...) to pictorial aesthetics. Two kinds of inflected seeing-in are distinguished in terms of two functions the design properties of a picture can realize. One kind of inflected seeing-in allows us to experience how the picture design sustains what is seen in the picture and is responsible for the representation of the picture subject. The second kind, which is only supported by some pictures, also captures how properties of the vehicle alter or enrich the picture content so as to elicit an experience of the depicted subject as having properties it could not be seen as having in face-to-face experience. This inflected experience is distinctively associated with our visual experience of the aesthetically valuable relations between vehicle and content which are unique to pictorial representation. (shrink)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  7
    Categorical Imperative and Ethical Inflection.Margit Ruffing, Guido A. De Almeida, Ricardo R. Terra & Valerio Rohden - 2008 - In Margit Ruffing, Guido A. De Almeida, Ricardo R. Terra & Valerio Rohden (eds.), Law and Peace in Kant's Philosophy/Recht und Frieden in der Philosophie Kants: Proceedings of the 10th International Kant Congress/Akten des X. Internationalen Kant-Kongresses. Walter de Gruyter.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  15
    Explaining uncertainty and defectivity of inflectional paradigms.Neil Bermel & Alexandre Nikolaev - 2022 - Cognitive Linguistics 33 (3):585-621.
    The current study investigates how native speakers of a morphologically complex language handle uncertainty related to linguistic forms that have gaps in their inflectional paradigms. We analyze their strategies of dealing with paradigmatic defectivity and how these strategies are motivated by subjective contemporaneousness, frequency, acceptability, and other lexical and structural characteristics of words. We administered a verb production task with Finnish native speakers using verbs from a small non-productive inflectional type that has many paradigmatic gaps and asked participants to inflect (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  37
    Problems of points of inflection in trend functions as described by a model for the forecast of brand shares.Werner Kroeber-Riel & Sighard Roloff - 1973 - Theory and Decision 3 (3):222-230.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  8
    Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by inflected adjectives.M. Gurjanov - 1985 - Cognition 19 (1):55-71.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43.  28
    Disentangling Effects of Input Frequency and Morphophonological Complexity on Children's Acquisition of Verb Inflection: An Elicited Production Study of Japanese.Tomoko Tatsumi, Ben Ambridge & Julian M. Pine - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (S2):555-577.
    This study aims to disentangle the often-confounded effects of input frequency and morphophonological complexity in the acquisition of inflection, by focusing on simple and complex verb forms in Japanese. Study 1 tested 28 children aged 3;3–4;3 on stative and simple past forms, and Study 2 tested 30 children aged 3;5–5;3 on completive and simple past forms, with both studies using a production priming paradigm. Mixed effects models for children's responses were built to test the prediction that children's verb use (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  14
    The stylistic inflection.Ian Buchanan - 1999 - Paragraph 22 (2):133-145.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  59
    Žižek's Meontology: An Inflected Hegel and the Possibility of Theology.Cyril O'Regan - 2014 - Modern Theology 30 (4):600-611.
  46.  30
    An Elicited‐Production Study of Inflectional Verb Morphology in Child Finnish.Sanna H. M. Räsänen, Ben Ambridge & Julian M. Pine - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (7):1704-1738.
    Many generativist accounts argue for very early knowledge of inflection on the basis of very low rates of person/number marking errors in young children's speech. However, studies of Spanish and Brazilian Portuguese have revealed that these low overall error rates actually hide important differences across the verb paradigm. The present study investigated children's production of person/number marked verbs by eliciting present tense verb forms from 82 native Finnish-speaking children aged 2;2–4;8 years. Four main findings were observed: Rates of person/number (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  47.  6
    On the compound Inflections Made with Verb “to be” and Their Uses in Turkey Turkish Accents.Özgür Ay - 2010 - Journal of Turkish Studies 6:679-692.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48. The nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection.Steven Pinker & Joseph Shimron - unknown
    Most evidence for the role of regular inflection as a default operation comes from languages that confound the morphological properties of regular and irregular forms with their phonological characteristics. For instance, regular plurals tend to faithfully preserve the base’s phonology, whereas irregular nouns tend to alter it. The distinction between regular and irregular inflection may thus be an epiphenomenon of phonological faithfulness. In Hebrew noun inflection, however, morphological regularity and phonological faithfulness can be distinguished: Nouns whose stems (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49.  12
    Crowdsourcing and Minority Languages: The Case of Galician Inflected Infinitives1.Michelle Sheehan, Martin Schäfer & Maria Carmen Parafita Couto - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Results from a crowdsourced audio questionnaire show that inflected infinitives in Galician are still acceptable in a broad range of contexts, different from those described for European Portuguese. Crucially, inflected infinitives with referential subjects are widely accepted only inside strong islands in Galician (complements of nouns, adjunct clauses). They are widely rejected in non-islands, notably in the complements of epistemic/factive verbs, in contrast with Portuguese and older varieties of Galician (Gondar 1978, Raposo 1987). Statistical analysis shows, however, that, in the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  25
    Innateness, autonomy, universality, and the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology.David Kemmerer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-641.
    Müller's goal of bringing neuroscience to bear on controversies in linguistics is laudable. However, some of his specific proposals about innateness and autonomy are misguided. Recent studies on the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology indicate that these two linguistic processes are subserved by anatomically and physiologically distinct neural subsystems, whose functional organization is likely to be under direct genetic control rather than assembled by strictly epigenetic factors.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 593