Results for 'muscular tensions'

998 found
Order:
  1.  13
    Muscular tension and the human blink rate.Donald C. King & Kenneth M. Michels - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (2):113.
  2.  28
    Induced muscular tension, incentive, and blink rate in a verbal learning task.Joseph B. Sidowski & Conrad Nuthmann - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (4):295.
  3.  20
    The role of muscular tension in the recall of interrupted tasks.D. W. Forrest - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (2):181.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  28
    Effect of experimentally induced muscular tension on psychomotor performance.Jack A. Adams - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (2):127.
  5.  18
    The relationship of induced muscular tension, tension level, and manifest anxiety in learning.O. Ivar Lovaas - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (3):145.
  6.  11
    Changes in muscular tension in coordinated hand movements.B. Johnson - 1928 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 11 (5):329.
  7.  12
    Changes in muscular tension during learning.C. W. Telford & W. J. Swenson - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 30 (3):236.
  8.  19
    The role of muscular tension in the comparison of lifted weights.B. Payne & R. C. Davis - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 27 (3):227.
  9.  13
    Changes in distribution of muscular tension during psychomotor performance.Lee W. Gregg - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (1):70.
  10.  13
    The role of muscular tensions in stylus maze learning.J. B. Stroud - 1931 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 14 (6):606.
  11.  19
    Changes in neuro-muscular tension accompanying the performance of a learning problem involving constant choice time.E. Ghiselli - 1936 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 19 (1):91.
  12.  20
    The effect of induced muscular tension upon various phases of the learning process.J. C. Stauffacher - 1937 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 21 (1):26.
  13.  22
    Apparatus for measuring muscular tensions.J. B. Stroud - 1931 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 14 (2):184.
  14.  17
    Effects of foreperiod, induced muscular tension, and stimulus regularity on simple reaction time.Warren H. Teichner - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (4):277.
  15.  32
    Relations between experimentally induced muscular tension and memorization.F. A. Courts - 1939 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 25 (3):235.
  16.  18
    The influence of muscular tension on the eyelid reflex.F. A. Courts - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 27 (6):678.
  17.  12
    Adaptation of the muscular tension response to gunfire.R. C. Davis & D. W. Van Liere - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (1):114.
  18.  8
    Compensatory reinforcements of muscular tension subsequent to sleep loss.G. L. Freeman - 1932 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 15 (3):267.
  19.  14
    Supplementary report: The relationship of induced muscular tension to manifest anxiety in learning.O. Ivar Lovaas - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (3):205.
  20.  27
    Summation of manifest anxiety and muscular tension.Donald R. Meyer & Merrill E. Noble - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (6):599.
  21.  12
    The influence of an increase in muscular tension on mental efficiency.Edna Nelson Zartman & Hulsey Cason - 1934 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (5):671.
  22.  13
    Effects of aspiration and achievement on muscular tensions.Saul S. Leshner - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (2):133.
  23.  8
    Performance in eyelid conditioning related to changes in muscular tension and physiological measures of emotionality.W. N. Runquist & K. W. Spence - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (6):417.
  24.  14
    Dr. Courts on the influence of muscular tension on the lid reflex.H. Peak - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 30 (6):515.
  25.  9
    The relation of respiration and reflex winking rates to muscular tension during motor learning.C. W. Telford & A. Storlie - 1946 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 36 (6):512.
  26.  20
    An attempt to appraise individual differences in level of muscular tension.M. A. Wenger - 1943 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 32 (3):213.
  27.  22
    The influence of practice on the dynamogenic effect of muscular tension.F. A. Courts - 1942 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 30 (6):504.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  16
    A correlation between illumination intensity and nervous muscular tension resulting from visual effort.M. Luckiesh & F. K. Moss - 1933 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 16 (4):540.
  29.  17
    The knee-jerk as a measure of muscular tension.F. A. Courts - 1939 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 24 (5):520.
  30.  19
    Sense of effort and sense of muscular tension.D. I. McCloskey - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):156-157.
  31.  4
    Effect of Experimentally-Induced Trunk Muscular Tensions on the Sit-to-Stand Task Performance and Associated Postural Adjustments.Alain Hamaoui & Caroline Alamini-Rodrigues - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  32.  16
    Effects of residual tension on output and energy expenditure in muscular work.L. H. Sharp - 1941 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 29 (1):1.
  33.  10
    The optimal locus of 'anticipatory tensions' in muscular work.G. L. Freeman - 1937 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 21 (5):554.
  34.  19
    Induced muscle tension and response shift in paired-associate learning.Irwin P. Levin - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (3):422.
  35.  19
    An action potential study of neuromuscular relations.S. R. Hathaway - 1935 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 18 (3):285.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  17
    The role of visual cues in the performance of ergographic work.Leonard E. Jarrard - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (1):57.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  80
    Effort awareness and sense of volition in schizophrenia.Gilles Lafargue & Nicolas Franck - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (1):277-289.
    Contemporary experimental research has emphasised the role of centrally generated signals arising from premotor areas in voluntary muscular force perception. It is therefore generally accepted that judgements of force are based on a central sense, known as the sense of effort, rather than on a sense of intra-muscular tension. Interestingly, the concept of effort is also present in the classical philosophy: to the French philosopher Maine de Biran [Maine de Biran . Mémoire sur la décomposition de la pensée (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  38.  10
    Diagonalized Asymmetry.Ari Santas - 2015 - International Journal of Applied Philosophy 29 (2):207-220.
    Myofascial Pain Syndrome is somatic pain due to muscular tension associated with muscular-skeletal imbalance. The pain and discomfort of the patient is not simply due to some isolated tension, but in the dynamic relationships between related structures. As the body adjusts to reestablish balance and symmetry, the tension and pain in one area “diagonalizes,” creating a tense correlate along a diagonal axis. This diagonalization of tension exacerbates and perpetuates the initial condition of pain and dysfunction. The purpose of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  33
    Monotonous Percussion Drumming and Trance Postures: A Controlled Evaluation of Phenomenological Effects.Lisa N. Woodside, V. K. Kumar & Ronald J. Pekala - 1997 - Anthropology of Consciousness 8 (2-3):69-87.
    Felicitas Goodman (1990) observed that naive participants experienced unique trance states, characterized by specific visionary content, when they assumed particular postures and listened to monotonous rattling. Students (n = 284), enrolled in various sections of the course Introduction to Psychology, experienced one of four conditions with their eyes closed: Sitting Quietly with and without Drumming, Standing (Feather Serpent) Posture plus Drumming with and without Suggested Experiences. Participants completed the Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (Pekala 1982, 1991c) and wrote narratives following their (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  40.  18
    To be held and to hold one’s own: narratives of embodied transformation in the treatment of long lasting musculoskeletal problems.Randi Sviland, Kari Martinsen & Målfrid Råheim - 2014 - Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 17 (4):609-624.
    This study elaborates on narrative resources emerging in the treatment of longlasting musculoskeletal and psychosomatic disorders in Norwegian psychomotor physiotherapy. Patients’ experiences produced in focus group interviews were analyzed from a narrative perspective, combining common themes across groups with in depth analysis of selected particular stories. NPMP theory expanded by Løgstrup’s and Ricoeur’s philosophy, and Mattingly’s and Frank’s narrative approach provided the theoretical perspective. Patients had discovered meaning imbued in muscular tension. Control shifted from inhibiting discipline and cognitive strategies, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  31
    The relationship under stress between changes in skin temperature, electrical skin resistance, and pulse rate.Lawrence M. Baker & William M. Taylor - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (5):361.
  42.  49
    Kinesthetic Understanding and Appreciation in Dance.William P. Seeley NoËl Carroll - 2013 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 71 (2):177-186.
    The idea that choreographic movements communicate to audiences by kinetic transfer is a commonplace among choreographers, dancers, and dance educators.1 Moreover, most dance lovers can cite their own favorite examples—the bounciness of the Royal Danish Ballet, the stomping of Bharata Natyam performers, the stag leaps in the thundering Greek chorus in Martha Graham’s Night Journey, or the contagious rhythmic transfer that takes over our feet when we watch classic tap dancers like Buster Brown. The perceptual capacity for kinetic transfer was (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43. Touch as a sense of force.Olivier Massin - manuscript
    The aim of this paper is to give a description of the objects of the sense of touch. Those objects, it is argued, are forces, rather than flesh deformation, solidity or weight. Tangible forces, basically tensions and pressures, are construed as symmetric and non-spatially reducible causal relations. Two consequences are drawn: first, the perception of heat and cold falls outside the sense of touch; second, muscular sense (together with a large part of proprioception) falls inside the sense of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44. Enkinaesthetic polyphony: the underpinning for first-order languaging.Susan A. J. Stuart & Paul J. Thibault - unknown
    We contest two claims: (1) that language, understood as the processing of abstract symbolic forms, is an instrument of cognition and rational thought, and (2) that conventional notions of turn-taking, exchange structure, and move analysis, are satisfactory as a basis for theorizing communication between living, feeling agents. We offer an enkinaesthetic theory describing the reciprocal affective neuro-muscular dynamical flows and tensions of co- agential dialogical sense-making relations. This “enkinaesthetic dialogue” is characterised by a preconceptual experientially recursive temporal dynamics (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  45.  4
    These Things I Believe.A. M. Shuham - 2013 - Narrative Inquiry in Bioethics 3 (2):120-122.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:These Things I BelieveA. M. ShuhamI am a health care professional who has worked in the field for two decades. I have been part of small miracles and heartbreaking events, which kept me up at night. Although I do not [End Page 120] provide direct patient care, my advanced education and expertise allows me to advise members of the health care team when difficult questions arise about the goals (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  7
    Experiencing Rhythm in Dance.John M. Wilson & Matthew Henley - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    In this article, two dance educators offer a definition of rhythm from both educational and performance perspectives and discuss pedagogical practices that waken students’ awareness to rhythm as a lived-experience over which they have creative control. For the dancer, in the midst of the dance, rhythms are, in the words of Margaret H’Doubler, recurring patterns of measured energy. These patterns are nested in scales from the moment-to-moment shifts in muscular contraction and release to the rise and fall of dramatic (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  20
    Muscular Imaginings—A Phenomenological and Enactive Model for Imagination.Jesús Ilundáin - 2017 - Sport, Ethics and Philosophy 11 (1):92-108.
    A phenomenological model is developed as an alternative to current analyses of the imagination in sport philosophy, heirs to an Enlightenment notion that conceptualizes imaginings as abstract, eidetic, and representational. EC describes how Eidetic and Corporeal Imaginings phenomenologically structure our imaginative undertakings. EIs keep the ‘ideal’ aspect, but CIs—enacted, corporeal, non-representational—are more fundamental and foundational. Sports are particularly suited to express CIs’ muscular imaginings, which result in novel performances. An enactive framework theorizes CIs as non-representational interactions.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  48.  31
    Muscular Imaginings—A Phenomenological and Enactive Model for Imagination.Jesús Ilundáin-Agurruza - 2017 - Sport, Ethics and Philosophy 11 (1):92-108.
    A phenomenological model is developed as an alternative to current analyses of the imagination in sport philosophy, heirs to an Enlightenment notion that conceptualizes imaginings as abstract, eidetic, and representational. EC describes how Eidetic and Corporeal Imaginings phenomenologically structure our imaginative undertakings. EIs keep the ‘ideal’ aspect, but CIs—enacted, corporeal, non-representational—are more fundamental and foundational. Sports are particularly suited to express CIs’ muscular imaginings, which result in novel performances. An enactive framework theorizes CIs as non-representational interactions.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  49.  16
    Muscular effort and electrodermal responses.Lawrence A. Pugh, Carl R. Oldroyd, Thomas S. Ray & Mervin L. Clark - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (2):241.
  50. Japanese Muscular Dystrophy Families Are More Accepting Of Fetal Diagnosis Than Patients.Darryl Macer & Hisanobu Kaiya - 1996 - Eubios Journal of Asian and International Bioethics 6 (4):103-104.
    A survey of members of the Japan Muscular Dystrophy Association , including patients and families, was conducted in October 1995. Some of the same questions that were included in a 1992 survey were included to allow comparisons. During the 1994 Annual Meeting in Kyoto a special session on fetal diagnosis and bioethics was included, attended by over 500 persons, which may have been a cause for increased awareness of genetic diagnosis seen in this survey. 60% of patients and 71% (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 998