Results for ' learning situation'

990 found
Order:
  1. Changing Practice.Situated Learning - 2008 - In Ash Amin & Joanne Roberts (eds.), Community, Economic Creativity, and Organization. Oxford University Press. pp. 283--296.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2. Learning situational awareness by observing expert actions.Taha A. Sidani & Avelino J. Gonzalez - forthcoming - Florida Ai Research Society Conference.
  3. Mental models in learning situations.N. M. Seel - 2006 - In Carsten Held, Markus Knauff & Gottfried Vosgerau (eds.), Mental Models and the Mind: Current Developments in Cognitive Psychology, Neuroscience, and Philosophy of Mind. Elsevier.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  4.  16
    Behavioral flexibility in learning situations: adaptive or adaptable behavior?Roderick Wong - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (1):81-82.
  5.  23
    Effect of first-order conditional probability in a two-choice learning situation.Norman H. Anderson - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (2):73.
  6. The Normativity Question in Quine’s Naturalism: The Context of Language Learning Situation.Shonkholen Mate - 2023 - Balkan Journal of Philosophy 15 (2):165-178.
    Quine has been charged with eliminating the normative dimension from his naturalized epistemology. The aim of the paper is to look at the role of empathy in Quine's language learning situation, which in its simplest form is constituted by the parent-child relation. We will explore the normativity of the role of empathy thereof by exploiting the sociality of the language learning situation. Since the sociality of Quine's notion of empathy is implicit, to explore the normativity expression (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  14
    Likelihood judgments and sequential effects in a two-choice probability learning situation.Norman H. Anderson & Richard E. Whalen - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (2):111.
  8.  26
    Comparison of predictions and estimates in a probability learning situation.E. D. Neimark & E. H. Shuford - 1959 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 57 (5):294.
  9.  19
    Application of a model for numerical response to a probability learning situation.Norman H. Anderson - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 80 (1):19.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  15
    Response probability in a two-choice learning situation with varying probability of reinforcement.Robert H. Hickson - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):138.
  11.  20
    Study of the Teachers’ Option of Primary and Preschool Education in Romania to Use Learning Situations for Training Pupils’ Skills.Aida Cornelia Stoian - 2016 - International Letters of Social and Humanistic Sciences 73:17-23.
    Publication date: 29 September 2016 Source: Author: Aida Cornelia Stoian This study highlights the results of an empirical research, whose purpose was to study the teachers` option of primary and preschool education to use learning situations for training pupils’ skills in relation to seniority in teaching and learning environment and to identify of the level of conceptual representation of the learning situations term in the opinion of teachers in primary and preschool education.We made a questionnaire in order (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  32
    The generation of conscious awareness in an incidental learning situation.Hilde Haider & Peter A. Frensch - 2005 - Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung 69 (5):399-411.
  13.  7
    “That’s going to be the hardest thing for me”: tensions experienced by quiet students during collaborative learning situations.Ann Medaille & Janet Usinger - 2018 - Educational Studies 46 (2):240-257.
    Collaborative learning has become increasingly prevalent throughout all disciplines in higher education, but these types of situations may prove challenging for quiet students who may prefer m...
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  16
    The effect of using differential end boxes in a simple T-maze learning situation.M. Ray Denny - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (3):245.
  15.  27
    The role of secondary reinforcement in a partial reinforcement learning situation.M. R. Denny - 1946 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 36 (5):373.
  16.  10
    Gradients of error reinforcement in normal multiple-choice learning situations.Melvin H. Marx - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (3):225.
  17.  9
    Elicitation theory: I. An analysis of two typical learning situations.M. Ray Denny & Harvey M. Adelman - 1955 - Psychological Review 62 (4):290-296.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  18.  15
    Event salience and response frequency on a ten-alternative probability-learning situation.Lee R. Beach & Richard W. Shoenberger - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (3):312.
  19. Cross-Situational Learning: An Experimental Study of Word-Learning Mechanisms.Kenny Smith, Andrew D. M. Smith & Richard A. Blythe - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (3):480-498.
    Cross-situational learning is a mechanism for learning the meaning of words across multiple exposures, despite exposure-by-exposure uncertainty as to the word's true meaning. We present experimental evidence showing that humans learn words effectively using cross-situational learning, even at high levels of referential uncertainty. Both overall success rates and the time taken to learn words are affected by the degree of referential uncertainty, with greater referential uncertainty leading to less reliable, slower learning. Words are also learned less (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  20.  8
    Vicarious reinforcement and limitation in a verbal learning situation.Robert E. Phillips - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (4p1):669.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  5
    Hypothesis analysis of conjunctive concept-learning situations.Lance A. Miller - 1971 - Psychological Review 78 (3):262-271.
  22. Promoting argumentation in face to face and in distributed computer-based learning situations: Constructing knowledge in the context of BioWorld.S. P. Lajoie - 1998 - In M. A. Gernsbacher & S. J. Derry (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawerence Erlbaum. pp. 5.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23. Cross‐Situational Learning of Phonologically Overlapping Words Across Degrees of Ambiguity.Karen E. Mulak, Haley A. Vlach & Paola Escudero - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (5):e12731.
    Cross‐situational word learning (XSWL) tasks present multiple words and candidate referents within a learning trial such that word–referent pairings can be inferred only across trials. Adults encode fine phonological detail when two words and candidate referents are presented in each learning trial (2 × 2 scenario; Escudero, Mulak, & Vlach, ). To test the relationship between XSWL task difficulty and phonological encoding, we examined XSWL of words differing by one vowel or consonant across degrees of within‐learning (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  24.  34
    Cross‐Situational Learning of Minimal Word Pairs.Paola Escudero, Karen E. Mulak & Haley A. Vlach - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (2):455-465.
    Cross-situational statistical learning of words involves tracking co-occurrences of auditory words and objects across time to infer word-referent mappings. Previous research has demonstrated that learners can infer referents across sets of very phonologically distinct words, but it remains unknown whether learners can encode fine phonological differences during cross-situational statistical learning. This study examined learners’ cross-situational statistical learning of minimal pairs that differed on one consonant segment, minimal pairs that differed on one vowel segment, and non-minimal pairs that (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  25.  14
    Cross‐situational Learning From Ambiguous Egocentric Input Is a Continuous Process: Evidence Using the Human Simulation Paradigm.Yayun Zhang, Daniel Yurovsky & Chen Yu - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (7):e13010.
    Recent laboratory experiments have shown that both infant and adult learners can acquire word‐referent mappings using cross‐situational statistics. The vast majority of the work on this topic has used unfamiliar objects presented on neutral backgrounds as the visual contexts for word learning. However, these laboratory contexts are much different than the real‐world contexts in which learning occurs. Thus, the feasibility of generalizing cross‐situational learning beyond the laboratory is in question. Adapting the Human Simulation Paradigm, we conducted a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  84
    Actively Learning Object Names Across Ambiguous Situations.George Kachergis, Chen Yu & Richard M. Shiffrin - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (1):200-213.
    Previous research shows that people can use the co-occurrence of words and objects in ambiguous situations (i.e., containing multiple words and objects) to learn word meanings during a brief passive training period (Yu & Smith, 2007). However, learners in the world are not completely passive but can affect how their environment is structured by moving their heads, eyes, and even objects. These actions can indicate attention to a language teacher, who may then be more likely to name the attended objects. (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  27.  42
    Learning Object Names at Different Hierarchical Levels Using Cross‐Situational Statistics.Chen Chi-Hsin, Zhang Yayun & Yu Chen - 2018 - Cognitive Science:591-605.
    Objects in the world usually have names at different hierarchical levels (e.g., beagle, dog, animal). This research investigates adults' ability to use cross‐situational statistics to simultaneously learn object labels at individual and category levels. The results revealed that adults were able to use co‐occurrence information to learn hierarchical labels in contexts where the labels for individual objects and labels for categories were presented in completely separated blocks, in interleaved blocks, or mixed in the same trial. Temporal presentation schedules significantly affected (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  28.  6
    Learning vocabulary and grammar from cross-situational statistics.Patrick Rebuschat, Padraic Monaghan & Christine Schoetensack - 2021 - Cognition 206 (C):104475.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  29.  89
    Learning Times for Large Lexicons Through Cross‐Situational Learning.Richard A. Blythe, Kenny Smith & Andrew D. M. Smith - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (4):620-642.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  30.  30
    Learning to Attend: A Connectionist Model of Situated Language Comprehension.Marshall R. Mayberry, Matthew W. Crocker & Pia Knoeferle - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (3):449-496.
    Evidence from numerous studies using the visual world paradigm has revealed both that spoken language can rapidly guide attention in a related visual scene and that scene information can immediately influence comprehension processes. These findings motivated the coordinated interplay account (Knoeferle & Crocker, 2006) of situated comprehension, which claims that utterance‐mediated attention crucially underlies this closely coordinated interaction of language and scene processing. We present a recurrent sigma‐pi neural network that models the rapid use of scene information, exploiting an utterance‐mediated (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  31. Situated learning in communities of practice. Resnick, L., Levine, J., Teasley, S., Eds.J. Lave - 1991 - In Lauren Resnick, Levine B., M. John, Stephanie Teasley & D. (eds.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. American Psychological Association.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  32.  21
    Rote learning as a function of distribution of practice and the complexity of the situation.Donald A. Riley - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (2):88.
  33.  53
    Competitive Processes in Cross‐Situational Word Learning.Daniel Yurovsky, Chen Yu & Linda B. Smith - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (5):891-921.
    Cross-situational word learning, like any statistical learning problem, involves tracking the regularities in the environment. However, the information that learners pick up from these regularities is dependent on their learning mechanism. This article investigates the role of one type of mechanism in statistical word learning: competition. Competitive mechanisms would allow learners to find the signal in noisy input and would help to explain the speed with which learners succeed in statistical learning tasks. Because cross-situational word (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  34.  7
    Cross‐Situational Word Learning With Multimodal Neural Networks.Wai Keen Vong & Brenden M. Lake - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (4).
    Cognitive Science, Volume 46, Issue 4, April 2022.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35.  19
    Discrimination learning in a verbal conditioning situation.Juliet Popper & Richard C. Atkinson - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 56 (1):21.
  36.  42
    Learning colour words is slow: A cross-situational learning account.Paul Vogt & Andrew D. M. Smith - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (4):509-510.
    Research into child language reveals that it takes a long time for children to learn the correct mapping of colour words. Steels & Belpaeme's (S&B's) guessing game, however, models fast learning of words. We discuss computational studies based on cross-situational learning, which yield results that are more consistent with the empirical child language data than those obtained by S&B.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  37.  97
    Situating learning in communities of practice.Jean Lave - 1991 - In Lauren Resnick, Levine B., M. John, Stephanie Teasley & D. (eds.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. American Psychological Association. pp. 2--63.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  38.  12
    Cross-situational learning in a Zipfian environment.Andrew T. Hendrickson & Amy Perfors - 2019 - Cognition 189 (C):11-22.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  39.  23
    Cross-situational and ostensive word learning in children with and without autism spectrum disorder.Courtney E. Venker - 2019 - Cognition 183 (C):181-191.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  40.  26
    Cross-situational and supervised learning in the emergence of communication.Jose Fernando Fontanari & Angelo Cangelosi - 2011 - Interaction Studies 12 (1):119-133.
    Scenarios for the emergence or bootstrap of a lexicon involve the repeated interaction between at least two agents who must reach a consensus on how to name N objects using H words. Here we consider minimal models of two types of learning algorithms: cross-situational learning, in which the individuals determine the meaning of a word by looking for something in common across all observed uses of that word, and supervised operant conditioning learning, in which there is strong (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  11
    Cross-situational and supervised learning in the emergence of communication.Jose Fernando Fontanari & Angelo Cangelosi - 2011 - Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies / Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systemsinteraction Studies 12 (1):119-133.
    Scenarios for the emergence or bootstrap of a lexicon involve the repeated interaction between at least two agents who must reach a consensus on how to name N objects using H words. Here we consider minimal models of two types of learning algorithms: cross-situational learning, in which the individuals determine the meaning of a word by looking for something in common across all observed uses of that word, and supervised operant conditioning learning, in which there is strong (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  7
    Current Situation and Strategy Formulation of College Sports Psychology Teaching Following Adaptive Learning and Deep Learning Under Information Education.Chuan Mou, Yi Tian, Fengrui Zhang & Chao Zhu - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    This study aims to explore the current situation and strategy formulation of sports psychology teaching in colleges and universities following adaptive learning and deep learning under information education. The informatization in physical education, teaching methods, and teaching processes make psychological education more scientific and efficient. First, the relevant theories of adaptive learning and deep learning are introduced, and an adaptive learning analysis model is implemented. Second, based on the deep learning automatic encoder, college (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  21
    Beyond situational meaning: From Dewey’s aesthetic experience to sensuous abstraction for deep learning.Qing Archer Zhang - forthcoming - Educational Philosophy and Theory.
    In his 1934 book Art as Experience John Dewey explores the relationship between human experience and art. His theory builds on the conception of experience inspired by Darwinian biology as the dyna...
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  20
    Situated (un-)learning in software design: a deconstructive approach.Roswitha Hofmann & Doris Allhutter - 2010 - Poiesis and Praxis 7 (1-2):87-98.
    Constructive technology assessment aims at anticipating societal impacts of technological innovations and suggests incorporating reflexivity and social learning into technology development. Social learning involves fostering the ability of diverse social actors to cultivate sociotechnical critical skills, thus allowing technological and social change to be governed with consideration for social values and diverging interests. Based on this demand, our paper presents a discourse-theoretical, interventionist approach to software design introducing deconstruction and (un-)learning as reflective practices to guide development processes. (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Situated learning, out of school and in the classroom.A. Renkl - 2001 - In N. J. Smelser & B. Baltes (eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. pp. 21--14133.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  43
    Learning strategies and situated knowledge.Antonio Rizzo & Oronzo Parlangeli - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):420-421.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Situated learning and changing practice.Jean Lave - 2008 - In Ash Amin & Joanne Roberts (eds.), Community, Economic Creativity, and Organization. Oxford University Press. pp. 283--296.
  48.  32
    Developmental Changes in Cross‐Situational Word Learning: The Inverse Effect of Initial Accuracy.Stanka A. Fitneva & Morten H. Christiansen - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S1):141-161.
    Intuitively, the accuracy of initial word-referent mappings should be positively correlated with the outcome of learning. Yet recent evidence suggests an inverse effect of initial accuracy in adults, whereby greater accuracy of initial mappings is associated with poorer outcomes in a cross-situational learning task. Here, we examine the impact of initial accuracy on 4-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults. For half of the participants most word-referent mappings were initially correct and for the other half most mappings were initially incorrect. Initial (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49. Re-Situating Learning.Karim Dharamsi - 2003 - Dissertation, University of Toronto (Canada)
    In this dissertation I examine the Theory-Theory . I argue that T-T represents the orthodox conception of learning in today's psychological literature. T-T theorists hold that human beings come "equipped" with innate representations that are "a theory." Theorists believe that this innate theory guides our relations to the world. If T-T theorists are correct, learning amounts to theory-revision. Hence, T-T brings together two commitments: innate knowledge and theory-revision. In this dissertation, I show that T-T depends on a reading (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  50.  25
    Learning and performance as a function of cs-intensity in a delayed gsr conditioning situation.J. F. Orlebeke & E. H. van Olst - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (3p1):483.
1 — 50 / 990