Results for 'Homo erectus'

1000+ found
Order:
  1. Was homo erectus an ecological dominant species?P. Slurink - 1995 - In John R. F. Bower & S. Sartono (eds.), Evolution and Ecology of Homo erectus. Pithecanthropus Centennial Foundation. pp. 169-176.
    Richard Alexander explains human uniqueness by postulating that at some point an ancestral species became 'ecological dominant' and the external forces of natural selection were replaced by within-species, intergroup competition. It is argued that this transition probably took place in archaic Homo sapiens (Homo heidelbergensis) and not in Homo erectus. Homo erectus was an ecological very flexible species, however.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  2. Did Homo erectus Dwell? Heidegger, Archaeology and the Future of Phenomenology.Philip Tonner - 2015 - In Paul J. Ennis & Tziovanis Georgakis (eds.), Heidegger in the Twenty-First Century. Dordrecht: Springer.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  31
    Locating early homo and homo erectus tool production along the extractive foraging/cognitive continuum.Sue Taylor Parker - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):414-415.
    This commentary contests Wynn's diagnosis of the cognitive implications of the earliest stone tools and Acheulian tools. I argue that the earliest stone tools imply greater cognitive abilities than those of great apes, and that Acheulian tools imply more than the preoperational cognitive abilities Wynn suggests. Finally, I suggest an alternative adaptive scenario for the evolution of hominid cognitive abilities.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  33
    Did Morality First Evolve in Homo erectus?Rappaport Margaret Boone & S. J. Christopher Corbally - 2016 - Zagadnienia Filozoficzne W Nauce 61:105-131.
    With findings from cognitive science, neuroscience, information science, and paleoanthropology, an anthropologist and astronomer-priest team take a new look at the nature of morality, and suggest parameters that are often very different from the philosophical and theological literatures. They see morality as a biologically-based arbitration mechanism that works along a timeline with a valence of good to bad. It is rational, purposeful, social, and affected by emotion but not dominated by it. The authors examine the age and sex structure, family (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  25
    Did Morality First Evolve in Homo erectus?Rappaport Margaret Boone & S. J. Christopher Corbally - 2016 - Philosophical Problems in Science 61:105-131.
    With findings from cognitive science, neuroscience, information science, and paleoanthropology, an anthropologist and astronomer-priest team take a new look at the nature of morality, and suggest parameters that are often very different from the philosophical and theological literatures. They see morality as a biologically-based arbitration mechanism that works along a timeline with a valence of good to bad. It is rational, purposeful, social, and affected by emotion but not dominated by it. The authors examine the age and sex structure, family (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  25
    Receiving an Ancestor in the Phylogenetic Tree: Neanderthal Man, Homo erectus and Homo floresiensis: L’histoire se Répète.John de Vos - 2009 - Journal of the History of Biology 42 (2):361-379.
    A comparison is made between the scientific receptions of three proposed new members of the hominin phylogenetic tree: the first finds of Neanderthal Man, those of Homo erectus, and those of Homo floresiensis. In each case, the leading scientists of the moment of discovery heavily debated the finds and neglected the meaning of those finds. At least it took/will take one generation before the meaning of those finds were/will be accepted.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7. I. Works by Nietzsche.Ecce Homo & All-too-Human Human - 2007 - In Brian Leiter & Neil Sinhababu (eds.), Nietzsche and morality. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 13--297.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Archaeology and cognitive evolution.Thomas Wynn - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):389-402.
    Archaeology can provide two bodies of information relevant to the understanding of the evolution of human cognition – the timing of developments, and the evolutionary context of these developments. The challenge is methodological. Archaeology must document attributes that have direct implications for underlying cognitive mechanisms. One example of such a cognitive archaeology is found in spatial cognition. The archaeological record documents an evolutionary sequence that begins with ape-equivalent spatial abilities 2.5 million years ago and ends with the appearance of modern (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  9.  15
    The human hearth and the dawn of morality.Margaret Boone Rappaport & Christopher Corbally - 2016 - Zygon 51 (4):835-866.
    Stunned by the implications of Colagè's analysis of the cultural activation of the brain's Visual Word Form Area and the potential role of cultural neural reuse in the evolution of biology and culture, the authors build on his work in proposing a context for the first rudimentary hominin moral systems. They cross-reference six domains: neuroscience on sleep, creativity, plasticity, and the Left Hemisphere Interpreter; palaeobiology; cognitive science; philosophy; traditional archaeology; and cognitive archaeology's theories on sleep changes in Homo (...) and consequences for later humans. The authors hypothesize that the human genome, when analyzed with findings from neuroscience and cognitive science, will confirm the evolutionary timing of an internal running monologue and other neural components that constitute moral decision making. The authors rely on practical modern philosophers to identify continuities with earlier primates, and one major discontinuity—some bright white moral line that may have been crossed more than once during the long and successful tenure of Homo erectus on Earth. (shrink)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  10.  33
    The vendantic absolute.Homo Leone - 1912 - Mind 21 (81):62-78.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11. Filozofia 2002. Č. 2.Homo Philosophicus - 2002 - Filozofia 57 (1-5):72.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  34
    The basic components of the human mind were not solidified during the Pleistocene epoch.Stephen M. Downes - 2010 - In Francisco José Ayala & Robert Arp (eds.), Contemporary debates in philosophy of biology. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 243–252.
    There are a number of competing hypotheses about human evolution. For example, Homo habilis and Homo erectus could have existed together, or one could have evolved from the other, and paleontological evidence may allow us to decide between these two hypotheses (see, e.g., Spoor et al., 2007). For most who work on the biology of human behavior, there is no question that human behavior is in some large part a product of evolution. But, there are competing hypotheses (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  13.  25
    Architectus.K. Pojmom Homo Architectus A. Deus - 2010 - Filozofia 65 (8):770.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14. the Meanings of" Humanism.V. R. Giustiniani & Humanus Homo - 1985 - Journal of the History of Ideas 46:175.
  15.  14
    Crowding Out and Crowding In of Intrinsic.Standard Microeconomics & Homo Oeconomicus - 2012 - In Eric Brousseau, Tom Dedeurwaerdere & Bernd Siebenhüner (eds.), Reflexive Governance for Global Public Goods. MIT Press. pp. 75.
  16. Beckett and Nietzsche: The Eternal Headache.Richard Lane & Ecce Homo - 2002 - In Richard J. Lane (ed.), Beckett and Philosophy. Palgrave. pp. 166.
  17.  8
    Webbed reprint collection.William Calvin - manuscript
    Homo erectus made symmetrical tools. The handaxe is a flattened tear-drop shape, but often with edges sharpened all around. Before we assign their obsession with symmetry to an esthetic judgment, we must consider whether it is possible that the symmetry is simply very pragmatic for one particular use of the many suggested.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  71
    Reasons for the preference for symmetry.Rolf Reber - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):415-416.
    Why did Homo erectus begin to craft symmetric tools? A parsimonious account assumes that preference for symmetry is inherent in all visual systems. This preference can be explained by a broader preference for perceptual fluency. The perceptual fluency account does not assume that selection for mate health or the production of symbolic art is a prerequisite for symmetry preference.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  19.  14
    Since when have humans had a soul?Andreas May - 2022 - HTS Theological Studies 78 (2).
    An attempt is made to determine when humans have had a soul. For this purpose, mind and soul are distinguished from each other. This clarification of terms makes it possible to criticise the emergentist view, which assumes that the soul arises naturally from the biological organism. The existence of a soul is inferred from the mental activities of humans, which are directed towards the transcendent. Special significance is given to burials. Burials have been practised for at least 448 000 years. (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  20. Précis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition.Merlin Donald - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):737-748.
    This bold and brilliant book asks the ultimate question of the life sciences: How did the human mind acquire its incomparable power? In seeking the answer, Merlin Donald traces the evolution of human culture and cognition from primitive apes to the era of artificial intelligence, and presents an original theory of how the human mind evolved from its presymbolic form. In the emergence of modern human culture, Donald proposes, there were three radical transitions. During the first, our bipedal but still (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   129 citations  
  21. From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness.Michael C. Corballis - 2003 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (2):199-208.
    The strong predominance of right-handedness appears to be a uniquely human characteristic, whereas the left-cerebral dominance for vocalization occurs in many species, including frogs, birds, and mammals. Right-handedness may have arisen because of an association between manual gestures and vocalization in the evolution of language. I argue that language evolved from manual gestures, gradually incorporating vocal elements. The transition may be traced through changes in the function of Broca's area. Its homologue in monkeys has nothing to do with vocal control, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   28 citations  
  22.  17
    Autocatalytic Closure in a Cognitive System: A Tentative Scenario for the Origin of Culture.L. Gabora - unknown
    This paper presents a speculative model of the cognitive mechanisms underlying the transition from episodic to mimetic (or memetic) culture with the arrival of Homo erectus, which Donald [1991] claims paved the way for the unique features of human culture. The model draws on Kauffman's [1993] theory of how an information-evolving system emerges through the formation of an autocatalytic network. Though originally formulated to explain the origin of life, this theory also provides a plausible account of how discrete (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  23. Cooperative feeding and breeding, and the evolution of executive control.Krist Vaesen - 2012 - Biology and Philosophy 27 (1):115-124.
    Dubreuil (Biol Phil 25:53–73, 2010b , this journal) argues that modern-like cognitive abilities for inhibitory control and goal maintenance most likely evolved in Homo heidelbergensis , much before the evolution of oft-cited modern traits, such as symbolism and art. Dubreuil’s argument proceeds in two steps. First, he identifies two behavioral traits that are supposed to be indicative of the presence of a capacity for inhibition and goal maintenance: cooperative feeding and cooperative breeding. Next, he tries to show that these (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  24.  37
    Recritude: Reflections on Postural Ontology.Adriana Cavarero - 2013 - Journal of Speculative Philosophy 27 (3):220-235.
    At the very beginning of the history of philosophy, with Plato, we are told the strange story of some men dwelling in a cave and looking at shadows. Immobilized by chains since childhood for all their lives, they are forced to sit on the cave's floor and are impeded from standing up. Hence, whoever they are and whatever they do, the label Homo erectus as a general category denoting the vertical posture of the human animal is decidedly unfit (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  25.  24
    Normativity, system-integration, natural detachment and the hybrid hominin.Lenny Moss - 2020 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 20 (1):21-37.
    From a subjective point of view, we take the existence of integrated entities, i.e., ourselves as the most unproblematic given, and blithely project such integrity onto untold many “entities” far and wide. However, from a naturalistic perspective, accounting for anything more integral than the attachments and attractions that are explicable in terms of the four fundamental forces of physics has been anything but straightforward. If we take it that the universe begins as an integral unity and explodes into progressive stages (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  12
    Human evolution.Bernard Wood - 1996 - Bioessays 18 (12):945-954.
    The common ancestor of modern humans and the great apes is estimated to have lived between 5 and 8 Myrs ago, but the earliest evidence in the human, or hominid, fossil record is Ardipithecus ramidus, from a 4.5 Myr Ethiopian site. This genus was succeeded by Australopithecus, within which four species are presently recognised. All combine a relatively primitive postcranial skeleton, a dentition with expanded chewing teeth and a small brain. The most primitive species in our own genus, Homo (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  27.  17
    Inclining Mimesis: Continuing the Dialogue with Adriana Cavarero.Nidesh Lawtoo & Adriana Cavarero - 2023 - Critical Horizons 24 (2):195-213.
    In this article, Adriana Cavarero and Nidesh Lawtoo resume a dialogue on mimetic inclinations in view of furthering a relational, embodied and affective conception of subjectivity that challenges homo erectus from the immanent perspective of homo mimeticus. If a dominant philosophical tradition tends to restrict mimesis to an illusory representation of reality, Plato was the first to know that mimesis also operates as an affective force, or pathos, that dispossesses the subject. While Plato tended to emphasize the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  3
    E. gendrolio bendruomeninės žmogaus raidos teorija.Gintautas Mažeikis - 2008 - Problemos 75.
    Straipsnyje analizuojami filosofo ir antropologo Edmundo Gendrolio filosofiniai ntropologiniai samprotavimai apie žmogaus savivokos, save suvokiančio mąstymo raidą. Gendrolis nuosekliai rėmėsi socialinės ir kultūrinės antropologijos teorijomis, empiriniais paleoantropologijos tyrinėjimais, etologijos prielaidomis. Straipsnio tikslas yra parodyti Gendrolio filosofinių antropologinių samprotavimų pecifiškumą, išskiriant jo bendruomeninės abstraktaus mąstymo kilmės teoriją, pabrėžiant solidarumo ir kultūros tvermės formų svarbą žmogaus raidai nuo seniausių Homo erectus laikų. Straipsnyje parodoma, kad Gendrolis nuosaikiai kritikavo marksistinę darbo teoriją, zoologinį individualizmą, linijinį evoliucionizmą, filosofinės antropologijos spekuliatyvumą, ir tvirtinama, kad (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29. The frontal feedback model of the evolution of the human mind: part 2, the human brain and the frontal feedback system.Raymond A. Noack - 2007 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 28 (3):233.
    The frontal feedback model argues that the sudden appearance of art and advancing technologies around 40,000 years ago in the hominid archaeological record was the end result of a recent fundamental change in the functional properties of the hominid brain, which occurred late in that brain's evolution. This change was marked by the switching of the driving mechanism behind the global, dynamic function of the brain from an "object-centered" bias, reflective of nonhuman primate and early hominid brains, to a "self-centered" (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  21
    Feeling Stone.Jeffrey Jerome Cohen - 2018 - Substance 47 (2):23-35.
    Stone hurts—and not simply because rocks so easily become missiles. The lithic offers a blunt challenge to our belief that humans matter. Homo sapiens are a species perhaps 200,000 years old. Homo erectus and Homo habilis, two of our earliest ancestors, go back perhaps 2.5 million years. That seems a substantial span. If you were to count one number per second and never pause to sleep or eat, it would take about twelve days to reach one (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  21
    The Human in the Light of Contemporary Biology as a Subject of Universal Civilization.Leszek Kuźnicki - 2005 - Dialogue and Universalism 15 (7-8):27-34.
    Homo sapiens is a mammal of the order Primates. What most distinguishes primates from other mammals is their ability to cerebrate. Cerebration developed fastest among the Anthropoidea primates , and subsequently the hominids . The increase in brain mass only by Homo sapiens—and only over the past 10,000 years—possess superior Darwinian fitness: for the preceding 30 million years primates had played a rather marginal role in the world’s biological system.Homo sapiens’ success as the creator of developed civilization (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  33
    The devil in the details.Thomas Wynn - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):426-432.
    Despite challenges on minimum necessary competence, intentionality, reliability, and context, the example of cognitive archaeology presented in the target article holds up well. The commentaries also present perspectives on cognition and symmetry that suggest an alternative to the target article's characterization of the cognitive abilities of early Homo erectus. However, the major conclusion of the initial argument – that the human ability to coordinate shape recognition and spatial cognition evolved hundreds of thousands of year ago in conditions unlike (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  61
    Semiotic hypercycles driving the evolution of language.Wolfgang Wildgen - 2008 - Axiomathes 18 (1):91-116.
    The evolution of human symbolic capacity must have been very rapid even in some intermediate stage (e.g. the proto-symbolic behavior of Homo erectus). Such a rapid process requires a runaway model. The type of very selective and hyperbolically growing self-organization called “hypercyle” by Eigen and Schuster could explain the rapidity and depth of the evolutionary process, whereas traditional runaway models of sexual selection seem to be rather implausible in the case of symbolic evolution. We assume two levels: at (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  71
    Biology Precedes, Culture Transcends: An Evolutionist's View of Human Nature.Francisco J. Ayala - 1998 - Zygon 33 (4):507-523.
    I will, first, outline what we currently know about the last 4 million years of human evolutionary history, from bipedal but small‐brained Australopithecus to modern Homo sapiens, our species, through the prolific toolmaker Homo habilis and the continent wanderer Homo erectus. I shall then identify anatomical traits that distinguish us from other animals and point out our two kinds of heredity, the biological and the cultural.Biological inheritance is based on the transmission of genetic information, in humans (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35.  23
    Language polygenesis: A probabilistic model.David A. Freedman & William Wang - unknown
    Monogenesis of language is widely accepted, but the conventional argument seems to be mistaken; a simple probabilistic model shows that polygenesis is likely. Other prehistoric inventions are discussed, as are problems in tracing linguistic lineages. Language is a system of representations; within such a system, words can evoke complex and systematic responses. Along with its social functions, language is important to humans as a mental instrument. Indeed, the invention of language,that is the accumulation of symbols to represent emotions, objects, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36. Mental attention, not language, may explain evolutionary growth of human intelligence and brain size.Juan Pascual-Leone - 2006 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29 (1):19-20.
    Using neoPiagetian theory of mental attention (or working memory), I task-analyze two complex performances of great apes and one symbolic performance (funeral burials) of early Homo sapiens. Relating results to brain size growth data, I derive estimates of mental attention for great apes, Homo erectus, Neanderthals, and modern Homo sapiens, and use children's cognitive development as reference. This heuristic model seems consistent with research.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  44
    Rediscovery and the cognitive aspects of toolmaking: Lessons from the handaxe.William H. Calvin - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):403-404.
    Long before signs of staged toolmaking appeared, Homo erectus made symmetrical tools. The handaxe is a flattened tear-drop shape, but often with edges sharpened all around. Before we assign their obsession with symmetry to an aesthetic judgment, we must consider whether it is possible that the symmetry is simply very pragmatic for one particular use in the many suggested.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  16
    The Unfolding of Language as Hysteron Proteron : Heterochrony and Extended Connectivity.Amadeu Viana - 2017 - Biosemiotics 10 (3):379-395.
    In this paper it is championed that a two stages hypothesis for the evolution of language must take into account a qualified approach to heterochrony and the available information from the archaeological record. As it seems, a protracted childhood and youth was already at work in Homo erectus, but early postnatal brain growth was only available to Homo sapiens. According to these facts, the term hysteron proteron is given here to the reversal that sets off linguistic capacity (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  11
    A Re-evaluation of the Androcentric Subject of European Philosophy.Willow Verkerk - 2023 - Critical Horizons 24 (2):115-130.
    This paper takes Cavarero’s arguments against the Homo erectus seriously and asks: how can we model an alternative to it? It proposes that a notion of the mimetically inclined subject is required, one that thickens Cavarero’s affirmative account of inclination by way of a new philosophical understanding of mimesis that includes habit and disciplinarity. Following Cavarero, the mother is positioned as a key figure to place nurturing and love at the centre of subject-making. However, they are shown to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  17
    Does cultural evolution need matriliny?Chris Knight - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2):339-340.
    Cetacean cultural transmission is associated with lengthened postmenopausal life histories and relatively stable matrilineal social structures. Although Homo erectus was not marine adapted, broadly comparable selection pressures, life history profiles, and social structures can be inferred.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  12
    Plio-pleistocene Hominids: Epistemological and Taxonomic Problems.Jolanta Koszteyn - 1970 - Forum Philosophicum: International Journal for Philosophy 9 (1):169-199.
    Within the historical times, which roughly corresponds with the Holocene epoch, the whole of mankind is believed to be a single species. Homo sapiens. But the human genealogical tree is populated by a really astounding number of paleontological species and paleontological genera: Ardipithecus ramidus, Australopithecus anamensis, Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus africanus, Paranthropus robustus, Paranthropus boisei, Homo habilis, Homo georgicus. Homo erectus, Homo ergaster, Homo antecessor, Homo heidelbergensis, Homo neanderthalensis, Homo sapiens.. In (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  4
    Plio-pleistocene Hominids: Epistemological and Taxonomic Problems.Jolanta Koszteyn - 1970 - Forum Philosophicum: International Journal for Philosophy 9 (1):169-202.
    Within the historical times, which roughly corresponds with the Holocene epoch, the whole of mankind is believed to be a single species. Homo sapiens. But the human genealogical tree is populated by a really astounding number of paleontological species and paleontological genera: Ardipithecus ramidus, Australopithecus anamensis, Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus africanus, Paranthropus robustus, Paranthropus boisei, Homo habilis, Homo georgicus. Homo erectus, Homo ergaster, Homo antecessor, Homo heidelbergensis, Homo neanderthalensis, Homo sapiens.. In (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  11
    Killing Our Way Out of Violence: Engaging Wrangham's The Goodness Paradox.Chris Haw & Richard Wrangham - 2022 - Contagion: Journal of Violence, Mimesis, and Culture 29 (1):63-99.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:Killing Our Way Out of ViolenceEngaging Wrangham's The Goodness ParadoxChris Haw (bio) and Richard Wrangham (bio)Wrangham's Goodness Paradox (GP) offers excellent anthropological research for mimetic theorists interested in the questions of human evolution and violence. It theorizes a framework of how group killing played a selective function in the emergence of our species, but it leaves open plenty of questions and concerns for productive, critical dialogue. Wolfgang Palaver has (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  30
    By the Waters of Babel: Jean-Louis Dessalles' Why We Talk.Fiona Cowie - 2010 - Biology and Philosophy 25 (5):880-888.
    Why We Talk is a complex, ambitious, original, thought-provoking, and sometimes frustrating book. In it, Jean-Louis Dessalles argues that the critical spur to the development of human language—language’s true biological function—was political. It wasn’t political in any of the senses hitherto floated in the literature, though: language didn’t evolve because it fostered group cohesion or cooperation, or facilitated mind-reading or manipulation. Instead, language originally served more or less the same function as ritualized displays of aggression and submission in many social (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  85
    Homo Sacer: Sovereign Power and Bare Life.Giorgio Agamben - 1998 - Stanford University Press.
    The work of Giorgio Agamben, one of Italy's most important and original philosophers, has been based on an uncommon erudition in classical traditions of philosophy and rhetoric, the grammarians of late antiquity, Christian theology, and modern philosophy. Recently, Agamben has begun to direct his thinking to the constitution of the social and to some concrete, ethico-political conclusions concerning the state of society today, and the place of the individual within it. In Homo Sacer, Agamben aims to connect the problem (...)
  46.  97
    Ecce homo.Friedrich Wilhelm Nietzsche & Raoul Richter - 1971 - [Paris]: Denoël/Gonthier. Edited by Anthony M. Ludovici.
    Published posthumously in 1908, Ecce Homo was written in 1888 and completed just a few weeks before Nietzsche’s complete mental collapse. Its outrageously egotistical review of the philosopher’s life and works—featuring chapters called Why I Am So Wise and Why I Write Such Good Books—are redeemed from mere arrogance by masterful language and ever-relevant ideas. In addition to settling scores with his many personal and philosophical enemies, Nietzsche emphasizes the importance of questioning traditional morality, establishing autonomy, and making a (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  47.  9
    Homo mundanus: jenseits der anthropischen Denkform der Moderne.Wolfgang Welsch - 2012 - Weilerswist: Velbrück.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  37
    Homo Technologicus: Threat or Opportunity?Kevin Warwick - 2016 - Philosophies 1 (3):199--208.
    Homo sapiens is entering a vital era in which the human-technology link is an inexorable trend. In this paper a look is taken as to how and why this is coming about and what exactly it means for both the posthuman species Homo technologicus and its originator Homo sapiens. Clearly moral and ethical issues are at stake. Different practical experimentation results that relate to the theme are described and the argument is raised as to why and how (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  49. Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences.Gerd Gigerenzer & Henry Brighton - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):107-143.
    Heuristics are efficient cognitive processes that ignore information. In contrast to the widely held view that less processing reduces accuracy, the study of heuristics shows that less information, computation, and time can in fact improve accuracy. We review the major progress made so far: the discovery of less-is-more effects; the study of the ecological rationality of heuristics, which examines in which environments a given strategy succeeds or fails, and why; an advancement from vague labels to computational models of heuristics; the (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   159 citations  
  50. Homo sacer.Giorgio Agamben - 1998 - Problemi 1.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   514 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000