Results for ' verbal instructions'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  31
    Verbal instructions targeting valence alter negative conditional stimulus evaluations.Camilla C. Luck & Ottmar V. Lipp - 2018 - Cognition and Emotion 32 (1):61-80.
    Negative conditional stimulus valence acquired during fear conditioning may enhance fear relapse and is difficult to remove as it extinguishes slowly and does not respond to the instruction that unconditional stimulus presentations will cease. We examined whether instructions targeting CS valence would be more effective. In Experiment 1, an image of one person was paired with an aversive US, while another was presented alone. After acquisition, participants were given positive information about the CS+ poser and negative information about the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  2.  11
    The Role of Verbal Instruction and Visual Guidance in Training Pattern Recognition.Jamie S. North, Ed Hope & A. Mark Williams - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  3.  68
    Effect of Verbal Instruction on Motor Learning Ability of Anaerobic and Explosive Exercises in Physical Education University Students.Souhail Hermassi, Maha Sellami, El Ghali Bouhafs, René Schwesig & Andrea De Giorgio - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  17
    Total time hypothesis in low-meaningful serial learning: Task, age and verbalization instructions.Elaine C. Koffman & Roy B. Weinstock - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (6):1210.
  5.  23
    Implicit memory and the enactment of verbal instructions.Lars-Göran Nilsson & Lars Bäckman - 1989 - In S. Lewandowsky, J. M. Dunn & K. Kirsner (eds.), Implicit Memory: Theoretical Issues. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 173--183.
  6. Learning Word Meaning From Dictionary Definitions: Sensorimotor Induction Precedes Verbal Instruction.Stevan Harnad - unknown
    Almost all words are the names of categories. We can learn most of our words (and hence our categories) from dictionary definitions, but not all of them. Some have to be learned from direct experience. To understand a word from its definition we need to already understand the words used in the definition. This is the “Symbol Grounding Problem” [1]. How many words (and which ones) do we need to ground directly in sensorimotor experience in order to be able to (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  12
    Verbal concept learning as a function of instructions and dominance level.Benton J. Underwood & Jack Richardson - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (4):229.
  8.  19
    Verbalization of mean field utterances in German instructions.O. I. Tayupova & O. N. Mashirenko - 2013 - Liberal Arts in Russia 2 (2):179--185.
    The article investigates ways of actualization of mean field utterances used in modern German instructions considering the type of the text. The author determines and analyzes similarities and differences in linguistic means used in mean field utterances in the context of such text subtypes as instructions to household appliances, cosmetic products directions and prescribing information for pharmaceutical drugs use.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  14
    Instructions to use verbal mediators in paired-associate learning.Marian Schwartz - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 79 (1p1):1.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  15
    Verbal discrimination learning and two-category classification learning as a function of list length and pronunciation instructions.John J. Shaughnessy - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (1):202.
  11.  35
    Verbal concept learning as a function of instructions and dominance level.E. B. Coleman - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (2):213.
  12.  25
    Instructions to use verbal mediators in learning a mixed paired-associate list.Marian Schwartz, Dennis C. Bunde, Richard W. Knitter & Paul D. Kottler - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (2):245.
  13.  27
    Imagery and verbal mediation instructions in paired-associate learning.John C. Yuille & Allan Paivio - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (3p1):436.
  14.  8
    Temporal connectives and verbal tenses as processing instructions.Cristina Grisot & Joanna Blochowiak - 2017 - Pragmatics and Cognition 24 (3):404-440.
    In this paper, we aim to enhance our understanding about the processing of implicit and explicit temporal chronological relations by investigating the roles of temporal connectives and verbal tenses, separately and in interaction. In particular, we investigate how two temporal connectives (ensuiteandpuis, both meaning ‘then’) and two verbal tenses expressing past time (the simple and compound past) act as processing instructions for chronological relations in French. Theoretical studies have suggested that the simple past encodes the instruction to (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  15.  12
    Verbalization of mean field utterances in German instructions.O. I. Tayupova & O. N. Mashirenko - 2013 - Liberal Arts in Russiaроссийский Гуманитарный Журналrossijskij Gumanitarnyj Žurnalrossijskij Gumanitaryj Zhurnalrossiiskii Gumanitarnyi Zhurnal 2 (2):179.
  16.  15
    Retention of verbal material as a function of motivating instructions and experimentally-induced failure.Wallace A. Russell - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (3):207.
  17.  15
    Role of instructions in two-choice verbal conditioning with contingent partial reinforcement.John Koehler - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (2):122.
  18.  25
    Verbal control of an autonomic response in a cue reversal situation.William W. Grings, Anne M. Schell & Cheryl A. Carey - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 99 (2):215.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  19.  26
    Using verbal protocol to examine construction of meaning from social studies texts.Kathryn L. Roberts & Kristy A. Brugar - 2022 - Journal of Social Studies Research 46 (2):135-151.
    Verbal protocol methodology is used to examine how fourth-grade students construct meaning as they read and respond to two informational social studies texts. Results indicate most students are active readers, often engaging in higher-level comprehension strategies and critical thinking as they read independently. However, critical thinking and comprehension processes are not often captured in their responses to end-of-reading questions (ERQ), which as a result have limited scope and utility for guiding social studies instruction. Results also indicate that when students (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  63
    Verbal and Behavioral Learning in a Probability Compounding Task.Daniel John Zizzo - 2003 - Theory and Decision 54 (4):287-314.
    The conjunction fallacy occurs whenever probability compounds are thought of as more likely than its component probabilities alone. In the experiment we present, subjects chose between simple and compound lotteries after some practice. Depending on the condition, they were given more or less information about the nature of probability compounds. The conjunction fallacy was surprisingly robust. There was, however, a puzzling dissociation between verbal and behavioral learning: verbal responses were sensitive, but actual choices entirely insensitive, to the amount (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  21. Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction.Camilla K. Gilmore, Shannon E. McCarthy & Elizabeth S. Spelke - unknown
    Symbolic arithmetic is fundamental to science, technology and economics, but its acquisition by children typically requires years of effort, instruction and drill1,2. When adults perform mental arithmetic, they activate nonsymbolic, approximate number representations3,4, and their performance suffers if this nonsymbolic system is impaired5. Nonsymbolic number representations also allow adults, children, and even infants to add or subtract pairs of dot arrays and to compare the resulting sum or difference to a third array, provided that only approximate accuracy is required6–10. Here (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  22.  33
    Instructions or dominion?: The meaning of the Spanish subjunctive mood.Rainer Vesterinen - 2013 - Pragmatics and Cognition 21 (2):359-379.
    In a highly interesting study, Dam and Dam-Jensen put forward the idea that the indicative and the subjunctive mood in Spanish complementizer phrases can be explained by the instructions they convey. The indicative instructs the addressee to locate the situation created by the verb relative to the situation of utterance, whereas the subjunctive instructs the addressee not to locate the situation described by the verb relative to the situation of utterance. Although this explanation is most appealing, the present paper (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23.  21
    Instructions or dominion?Rainer Vesterinen - 2013 - Pragmatics and Cognition 21 (2):359-379.
    In a highly interesting study, Dam and Dam-Jensen (2010) put forward the idea that the indicative and the subjunctive mood in Spanish complementizer phrases can be explained by the instructions they convey. The indicative instructs the addressee to locate the situation created by the verb relative to the situation of utterance, whereas the subjunctive instructs the addressee not to locate the situation described by the verb relative to the situation of utterance. Although this explanation is most appealing, the present (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  15
    Transfer in the W1-R2 verbal discrimination paradigm as a function of instructions.N. Jack Kanak & Zulekha Mehta - 1977 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 10 (6):493-495.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  11
    A multinomial modelling approach to face identity recognition during instructed threat.Nina R. Arnold, Hernán González Cruz, Sabine Schellhaas & Florian Bublatzky - 2021 - Cognition and Emotion 35 (7):1302-1319.
    To organise future behaviour, it is important to remember both the central and contextual aspects of a situation. We examined the impact of contextual threat or safety, learned through verbal instructions, on face identity recognition. In two studies (N = 140), 72 face–context compounds were presented each once within an encoding session, and an unexpected item/source recognition task was performed afterwards (including 24 new faces). Hierarchical multinomial processing tree modelling served to estimate individual parameters of item (face identity) (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  26.  49
    Retention of visual and verbal codes of the same stimuli.Harry P. Bahrick & Barbara Boucher - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (3p1):417.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  27.  19
    Instruction and Practice in Learning to use a Device.Peter A. Bibby & Stephen J. Payne - 1996 - Cognitive Science 20 (4):539-578.
    We explore the extent to which Anderson's (1987) theory of knowledge compilation can account for the relationship between instructions and practice in learning to use a simple device. Bibby and Payne (1993) reported experimental support for knowledge compilation in this domain. This article replicates the finding of a performance cross‐over between instruction type and task type that disappears with practice on the tasks. The research is extended by using verbal protocols to model the strategies of novice and more (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  28.  21
    Type of Instructional Material, Cognitive Style and Learning Performance.Richard Riding & Eugene Sadler‐Smith - 1992 - Educational Studies 18 (3):323-340.
    Summary The positions of 129 14 to 19?year?old students on two fundamental cognitive styles dimensions (Wholist?Analytic and Verbal?Imagery) were assessed. They then received, by random allocation, one of three versions of a computer?presented instruction package on home hot water systems. The versions differed in terms of their structure (large versus small step), advance organiser (absent or present), verbal emphasis (high versus low), and diagram type (abstract versus pictorial). Version 1 had large step, no organiser, high verbal content, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  29.  19
    Enhanced Instructed Fear Learning in Delusion-Proneness.Anaïs Louzolo, Rita Almeida, Marc Guitart-Masip, Malin Björnsdotter, Alexander Lebedev, Martin Ingvar, Andreas Olsson & Predrag Petrovic - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Psychosis is associated with distorted perceptions and deficient bottom-up learning such as classical fear conditioning. This has been interpreted as reflecting imprecise priors in low-level predictive coding systems. Paradoxically, overly strong beliefs, such as overvalued beliefs and delusions, are also present in psychosis-associated states. In line with this, research has suggested that patients with psychosis and associated phenotypes rely more on high-order priors to interpret perceptual input. In this behavioural and fMRI study we studied two types of fear learning, i.e., (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  12
    The Intelligibility of Haptic Perception in Instructional Sequences: When Visually Impaired People Achieve Object Understanding.Brian L. Due & Louise Lüchow - 2023 - Human Studies 46 (1):163-182.
    In this paper, we study the interactional organization of an instructed object exploration among sighted and visually impaired people (VIPs) in order to contribute to studies of instructional activities and the observable accomplishment of haptic perception. We do this by showing the situated, interactional, and co-operative organization of achieving object understanding. We focus on the dynamics of haptic perception as being reliant on instructions, while at the same time being an observable production that furnishes further instructions. We show (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  25
    Imagery and frequency processes in verbal discrimination learning.Edward J. Rowe - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 95 (1):140.
  32.  84
    Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal descriptions.Trygg Engen & Bruce M. Ross - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 100 (2):221.
  33. A multimodal contrastive analysis of regulations and instructions during the COVID-19 lockdown in the context of the Island of Madeira and the United Kingdom.Leandro da Silva & Svetlana Kurteš - forthcoming - Lodz Papers in Pragmatics.
    The paper analyses aspects of multilingual and multimodal narratives employed during the COVID-19 lockdown, attempting to identify how they were used in professional and public environments. More specifically, the paper looks at editing choices, photography, design, drawing, colour, and writing to get a better understanding of multimodal communication, semiotics, image analysis, and their correlation with the written text and the way it has been perceived and interpreted, be it in online or print-based contexts. Methodologically, this is a contrastively inspired research (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  19
    From potential to practice: Compelling questions as an impetus for curricular and instructional change.Rebecca G. W. Mueller - 2018 - Journal of Social Studies Research 42 (3):249-260.
    Despite arguments that successful inquiry hinges on an engaging question, relatively little attention has been paid to how teachers craft such questions. This study examined how six high school civics teachers defined and developed compelling questions and evaluated the potential of compelling questions to influence curriculum and instruction. This study used qualitative research methods and generated data through interviews, verbal reports, and content analysis of teacher-completed materials. Findings suggest that teachers are hopeful that compelling questions will prompt challenging instruction (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  35.  37
    Street Signs and Ikea Instruction Sheets: Pragmatics and Pictorial Communication.Marcello Frixione & Antonio Lombardi - 2015 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6 (1):133-149.
    A classical objection to pictorial communication is that pictures are intrinsically ambiguous and a picture, per se, can communicate an indeterminate number of different contents. The standard interpretation of this objection is that pictures are subordinate to language and that pictorial communication is parasitic on verbal communication. We argue that in many cases verbal communication presents a similar indeterminacy, which is resolved by resorting to pragmatic mechanisms. In this spirit, we propose a pragmatic approach which explains pictorial communication (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  8
    Two Types of Demonstration Through Guided Touch with Cane: Instruction Sequences in Orientation and Mobility Training for a Person with Visual Impairments.Yasusuke Minami, Hiro Yuki Nisisawa, Mitsuhiro Okada & Rui Sakaida - 2023 - Human Studies 46 (4):723-756.
    Persons with visual impairments (hereafter PVI) detect and discover obstacles and road conditions by touching with a white cane when walking on the streets. In one training session, an Orientation and Mobility specialist (hereafter SPT) guided a PVI by grasping and moving the cane that the PVI was holding. We conducted a multimodal analysis of two instruction sequences, one a "proving and achieving" demonstration (Sacks in Lectures on conversation, Blackwell, 1992) and the other a "learnable" (Zemel and Koschmann, in Discourse (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  33
    Going to the zoo: The role of gaze and other non-verbal behavior in task-based interactions.Gerardine M. Pereira - 2013 - Pragmatics and Cognition 21 (2):380-398.
    This paper reports on an investigation of gaze patterns and other non-verbal behavior in dyadic, problem-solving based interactions. In a planning activity, participants are given an instruction sheet and a physical map of a zoo. Both participants must coordinate their actions to find a common solution to the problem. This paper aims at examining how activity-based interactions vary from other interactions, such as everyday conversation and story-telling. The findings of this paper suggest that participants’ non-verbal behavior, such as (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  28
    Mnemonic transformations and verbal coding processes.Raymond W. Kulhavy & James R. Heinen - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (1):173.
  39.  12
    Children’s Spontaneous Gestures Reflect Verbal Understanding of the Day/Night Cycle.Caroline M. Gaudreau, Florencia K. Anggoro & Benjamin D. Jee - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Understanding the day/night cycle requires integrating observations of the sky (an Earth-based perspective) with scientific models of the solar system (a space-based perspective). Yet children often fail to make the right connections and resort to non-scientific intuitions – for example, the Sun moving up and down – to explain what they observe. The present research explored whether children’s gestures indicate their conceptual integration of Earth- and space-based perspectives. We coded the spontaneous gestures of 85 third-grade children in U.S. public schools (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  22
    Transfer of implicit associative responses between free-recall learning and verbal discrimination learning tasks.Lawrence E. Cole & N. Jack Kanak - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 95 (1):110.
  41.  7
    Rosenzweig and Bakhtin. Hermeneutics of Language and Verbal Art in the System of the Philosophy of Dialogue.Ilya Dvorkin - 2022 - RUDN Journal of Philosophy 26 (3):537-556.
    For all the differences in the teachings and fate of Franz Rosenzweig and Mikhail Bakhtin, comparing them with one another is extremely instructive and reveals important and often lost meanings of 20th-century philosophy. Bakhtin made his debut in 1929 as the author of Problems of Dostoevsky’s Creative Art, but then went into exile for sufficient years and emerged from oblivion only in the 1960s. Rosenzweig died in 1929 and was almost forgotten for many years. Now, almost a century later, we (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  7
    Sports Coaches’ Knowledge and Beliefs About the Provision, Reception, and Evaluation of Verbal Feedback.Robert J. Mason, Damian Farrow & John A. C. Hattie - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Coach observation studies conducted since the 1970s have sought to determine the quantity and quality of verbal feedback provided by coaches to their athletes. Relatively few studies, however, have sought to determine the knowledge and beliefs of coaches that underpin this provision of feedback. The purpose of the current study was to identify the beliefs and knowledge that elite team sport coaches hold about providing, receiving and evaluating feedback in their training and competition environments. Semi-structured interviews conducted with 8 (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  16
    Transfer tests of the frequency theory of verbal discrimination learning.David C. Raskin, Carol Boice & Edwin W. Rubel - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (4p1):521.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  28
    Effect of semantic redundancy on children's identification of verbal concepts.Francis J. Di Vesta & Gary M. Ingersoll - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (2):360.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  58
    Temporal binding: digging into animal minds through time perception.Antonella Tramacere & Colin Allen - 2022 - Synthese 200 (1):1-24.
    Temporal binding is the phenomenon in which events related as cause and effect are perceived by humans to be closer in time than they actually are). Despite the fact that temporal binding experiments with humans have relied on verbal instructions, we argue that they are adaptable to nonhuman animals, and that a finding of temporal binding from such experiments would provide evidence of causal reasoning that cannot be reduced to associative learning. Our argument depends on describing and theoretically (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  46.  33
    Bending rules: the shape of the perceptual generalisation gradient is sensitive to inference rules.Yannick Boddez, Marc Patrick Bennett, Silke van Esch & Tom Beckers - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (7):1444-1452.
    Generalising what is learned about one stimulus to other but perceptually related stimuli is a basic behavioural phenomenon. We evaluated whether a rule learning mechanism may serve to explain such generalisation. To this end, we assessed whether inference rules communicated through verbal instructions affect generalisation. Expectancy ratings, but not valence ratings, proved sensitive to this manipulation. In addition to revealing a role for inference rules in generalisation, our study has clinical implications as well. More specifically, we argue that (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  47.  7
    Putting Attention on the Spot in Coaching: Shifting to an External Focus of Attention With Imagery Techniques to Improve Basketball Free-Throw Shooting Performance.Kyle R. Milley & Gene P. Ouellette - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Attentional focus is an area that has garnered considerable attention in the sport psychology and motor performance literature. This is unsurprising given that attentional focus has been directly linked to performance outcomes and is susceptible to coaching input. While research has amassed supporting benefits of an external focus of attention on motor performance using verbal instruction, other studies have challenged the notion that an EFA is more beneficial than an internal focus of attention for sport-related performance. Further, it is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  48.  31
    Why it is so hard to teach people they can make a difference: climate change efficacy as a non-analytic form of reasoning.Matthew J. Hornsey, Cassandra M. Chapman & Dexter M. Oelrichs - 2022 - Thinking and Reasoning 28 (3):327-345.
    People who believe they have greater efficacy to address climate change are more likely to engage in pro-environmental behaviour. To confront the climate crisis, it will therefore be essential to understand the processes through which climate change efficacy is promoted. Some interventions in the literature assume that efficacy emerges from analytic reasoning processes: that it is deliberative, verbal, conscious, and influenced by information and education. In the current paper, we critique this notion. We review evidence showing that climate change (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  49.  25
    Measuring the spatial distribution of the metaattentional spotlight.Jun-Ichiro Kawahara - 2010 - Consciousness and Cognition 19 (1):107-124.
    Studies in cognitive psychology have shown that the deployment of visual attention operates under spatial limitations, rendering its assignment to multiple locations difficult or costly. This study explored whether this conventional understanding applies to human metaattention as well. I measured the spatial distribution of metaattention during viewing of natural scenes and found that participants believed they could attend to multiple locations simultaneously. Study 2 tested whether this tendency could be modified by information about the tendency to overestimation. After participants were (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  50.  53
    Automatic Mechanisms for Social Attention Are Culturally Penetrable.Adam S. Cohen, Joni Y. Sasaki, Tamsin C. German & Heejung S. Kim - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (1):242-258.
    Are mechanisms for social attention influenced by culture? Evidence that social attention is triggered automatically by bottom-up gaze cues and is uninfluenced by top-down verbal instructions may suggest it operates in the same way everywhere. Yet considerations from evolutionary and cultural psychology suggest that specific aspects of one's cultural background may have consequence for the way mechanisms for social attention develop and operate. In more interdependent cultures, the scope of social attention may be broader, focusing on more individuals (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000