Results for ' stimulus dimensionality'

1000+ found
Order:
  1.  4
    Stimulus dimensionality and discrimination learning in elementary schoolchildren.C. F. Etaugh & B. R. Ankney - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (2):110-111.
  2.  11
    The influence of stimulus dimensionality and viewing orientation on detection of symmetry in dot patterns.Paul Locher & Gerda Smets - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (1):43-46.
  3.  28
    Partial advance information and stimulus dimensionality.Barry H. Kantowitz & Mark S. Sanders - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):412.
  4.  17
    Sensitivity to apparent movement in depth as a function of stimulus dimensionality.William M. Smith - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (2):149.
  5.  23
    Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy.Sylvan Kornblum, Thierry Hasbroucq & Allen Osman - 1990 - Psychological Review 97 (2):253-270.
  6.  25
    "Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--A model and taxonomy": Correction to Kornblum et al.Sylvan Kornblum, Thierry Hasbroucq & Allen Osman - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):44-44.
  7.  16
    Effects of stimulus novelty and dimensional saliency in human shift learning.Nicholas J. Esposito - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (2):264.
  8.  21
    Redundancy and dimensional load in stimulus identification.Robert M. Levy - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 98 (1):138.
  9.  14
    Ordinal position effects with a two-dimensional stimulus array.Robert K. Young & Richard E. Buck - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (1):161.
  10.  28
    Choices based on redundant information: An analysis of two-dimensional stimulus control.Sheila Chase & Eric G. Heinemann - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (2):161.
  11.  11
    Spatial compatibility with a two-dimensional stimulus arrangement.E. Soetens, M. Deboeck, J. Hueting & H. Merckx - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (2):125-128.
  12.  26
    Only stimulus energy affects the detectability of visual forms and objects.Muriel Boucart & Claude Bonnet - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (5):415-417.
    A detection task was performed using different pictographic representations of objects in order to test the hypothesis that high-level information (familiarity) may influence detection thresholds. The stimuli were five versions of forms: outline drawings of objects, silhouettes, and three fragmented versions of forms derived from the outlines. The stimuli varied on two parameters: their nameability (easily nameable, hardly nameable, and not nameable) as assessed by a naming task, and their energy content as assessed by a two-dimensional fast-Fourier transform. The greatest (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13.  64
    Two-dimensional symmetric form discrimination: Fast learning, but notthat fast.Ivans Chou & Lucia M. Vaina - 1995 - Synthese 104 (1):33 - 41.
    Several authors have characterized a striking phenomenon of perceptual learning in visual discrimination tasks. This learning process is selective for the stimulus characteristics and location in the visual field. Since the human visual system exploits symmetry for object recognition we were interested in exploring how it learns to use preattentive symmetry cues for discriminating simple, meaningless, forms. In this study, similar to previous studies of perceptual learning, we asked whether the effects of practice acquired in the discrimination of pairs (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14.  42
    Some determinants of two-dimensional visual tracking behavior.Jack A. Adams & Louis V. Xhignesse - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (6):391.
  15.  11
    Cognitive Control and Bilingualism: The Bilingual Advantage Through the Lens of Dimensional Overlap.Melodie Bellegarda & Pedro Macizo - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Past research shows that the bilingual experience may enhance cognitive executive function. In this experiment, we evaluated cognitive control in bilinguals relative to monolinguals by using a dimensional overlap model to predict performance in a task composed of Stroop and Simon stimuli. A group of 24 Spanish monolinguals and 24 bilinguals with differing first languages and all having Spanish as a second language did a picture naming task and a task composed of Stroop and Simon stimuli, where the effect of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  85
    The development of features in object concepts.Philippe G. Schyns, Robert L. Goldstone & Jean-Pierre Thibaut - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (1):1-17.
    According to one productive and influential approach to cognition, categorization, object recognition, and higher level cognitive processes operate on a set of fixed features, which are the output of lower level perceptual processes. In many situations, however, it is the higher level cognitive process being executed that influences the lower level features that are created. Rather than viewing the repertoire of features as being fixed by low-level processes, we present a theory in which people create features to subserve the representation (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   98 citations  
  17. The Sensory Core and the Medieval Foundations of Early Modern Perceptual Theory.Gary Hatfield & William Epstein - 1979 - Isis 70 (3):363-384.
    This article seeks the origin, in the theories of Ibn al-Haytham (Alhazen), Descartes, and Berkeley, of two-stage theories of spatial perception, which hold that visual perception involves both an immediate representation of the proximal stimulus in a two-dimensional ‘‘sensory core’’ and also a subsequent perception of the three dimensional world. The works of Ibn al-Haytham, Descartes, and Berkeley already frame the major theoretical options that guided visual theory into the twentieth century. The field of visual perception was the first (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  18. Depth perception from pairs of overlapping cues in pictorial displays.Birgitta Dresp, Severine Durand & Stephen Grossberg - 2002 - Spatial Vision 15:255-276.
    The experiments reported herein probe the visual cortical mechanisms that control near–far percepts in response to two-dimensional stimuli. Figural contrast is found to be a principal factor for the emergence of percepts of near versus far in pictorial stimuli, especially when stimulus duration is brief. Pictorial factors such as interposition (Experiment 1) and partial occlusion Experiments 2 and 3) may cooperate, as generally predicted by cue combination models, or compete with contrast factors in the manner predicted by the FACADE (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  19.  85
    Visual motion disambiguation by a subliminal sound.Andre Dufour, Pascale Touzalin, Michèle Moessinger, Renaud Brochard & Olivier Després - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):790-797.
    There is growing interest in the effect of sound on visual motion perception. One model involves the illusion created when two identical objects moving towards each other on a two-dimensional visual display can be seen to either bounce off or stream through each other. Previous studies show that the large bias normally seen toward the streaming percept can be modulated by the presentation of an auditory event at the moment of coincidence. However, no reports to date provide sufficient evidence to (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  20. Seeing, visualizing, and believing: Pictures and cognitive penetration.John Zeimbekis - 2015 - In John Zeimbekis & Athanassios Raftopoulos (eds.), The Cognitive Penetrability of Perception: New Philosophical Perspectives. Oxford University Press. pp. 298-327.
    Visualizing and mental imagery are thought to be cognitive states by all sides of the imagery debate. Yet the phenomenology of those states has distinctly visual ingredients. This has potential consequences for the hypothesis that vision is cognitively impenetrable, the ability of visual processes to ground perceptual warrant and justification, and the distinction between cognitive and perceptual phenomenology. I explore those consequences by describing two forms of visual ambiguity that involve visualizing: the ability to visually experience a picture surface as (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  21. Obstacles to Testing Molyneux's Question Empirically.Tony Cheng - 2015 - I-Perception 6 (4).
    There have recently been various empirical attempts to answer Molyneux’s question, for example, the experiments undertaken by the Held group. These studies, though intricate, have encountered some objections, for instance, from Schwenkler, who proposes two ways of improving the experiments. One is “to re-run [the] experiment with the stimulus objects made to move, and/or the subjects moved or permitted to move with respect to them” (p. 94), which would promote three dimensional or otherwise viewpoint-invariant representations. The other is “to (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  22.  15
    テキストデータを用いた類義語の自動作成.稲子 希望 笠原 要 - 2003 - Transactions of the Japanese Society for Artificial Intelligence 18 (4):221-232.
    A method of generating synonyms for a stimulus word using a computer is proposed. Vector Space Model, where words in text data are arranged in a multi-dimensional space and degree of similarity between two words of them is calculated from how close the words are in the space, may be available to the method. However, it is not easy to optimize parameters in the method because there is no appropriate standard synonym database where proper synonyms for a stimulus (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  23. Infants' discrimination of number vs. continuous extent.Elizabeth Spelke - manuscript
    Seven studies explored the empirical basis for claims that infants represent cardinal values of small sets of objects. Many studies investigating numerical ability did not properly control for continuous stimulus properties such as surface area, volume, contour length, or dimensions that correlate with these properties. Experiment 1 extended the standard habituation/dishabituation paradigm to a 1 vs 2 comparison with three-dimensional objects and confirmed that when number and total front surface area are confounded, infants discriminate the arrays. Experiment 2 revealed (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  24.  9
    What Is the Effect of Basic Emotions on Directed Forgetting? Investigating the Role of Basic Emotions in Memory.Artur Marchewka, Marek Wypych, Jarosław M. Michałowski, Marcin Sińczuk, Małgorzata Wordecha, Katarzyna Jednoróg & Anna Nowicka - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10:202287.
    Studies presenting memory-facilitating effect of emotions typically focused on affective dimensions of arousal and valence. Little is known, however, about the extent to which stimulus-driven basic emotions could have distinct effects on memory. In the present paper we sought to examine the modulatory effect of disgust, fear and sadness on intentional remembering and forgetting using widely used item-method directed forgetting paradigm. Eighteen women underwent fMRI scanning during encoding phase in which they were asked either to remember (R) or to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  25.  5
    Distinguishing absence of awareness from awareness of absence.Matan Mazor & Stephen M. Fleming - 2020 - Philosophy and the Mind Sciences 1 (II).
    Contrasting brain states when subjects are aware compared to unaware of a presented stimulus has allowed researchers to isolate candidate neural correlates of consciousness. Here we propose that an important next step in this research program is to investigate, perhaps paradoxically, brain states that covary with reports of absences of awareness. Specifically, we propose that in order to distinguish content-specific and content-invariant neural correlates of consciousness, a distinction needs to be made between the neural correlates of awareness of (...) absence, and the neural correlates of absence of awareness. We ground this distinction in higher-order computational models of consciousness, where the state of higher-order nodes is invariant to the specific contents of awareness. To map the different levels of these models to neurophysiological correlates, we suggest two empirical approaches – inverted designs and two-dimensional awareness reports – in which reports about awareness and stimulus presence can be dissociated. (shrink)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  26. Reconsidering the mind-wandering reader: predictive processing, probability designs, and enculturation.Regina Fabry & Karin Kukkonen - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:1-14.
    Studies on mind-wandering frequently use reading as an experimental task. In these studies, reading is conceived as a cognitive process that potentially offers a contrast to mind-wandering, because it seems to be task-related, goal-directed and stimulus-dependent. More recent work attempts to avoid the dichotomy of successful cognitive processes and processes of mind-wandering found in earlier studies. We approach the issue from the perspective that texts provoke modes of cognitive involvement different from the information processing and recall account that underlies (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  80
    Shape, perspective, and what is and is not perceived: Comment on Morales, Bax, and Firestone (2020).Johannes Burge & Tyler Burge - 2023 - Psychological Review 130 (4):1125-1136.
    Psychology and philosophy have long reflected on the role of perspective in vision. Since the dawn of modern vision science—roughly, since Helmholtz in the late 1800s—scientific explanations in vision have focused on understanding the computations that transform the sensed retinal image into percepts of the three-dimensional environment. The standard view in the science is that distal properties—viewpoint-independent properties of the environment (object shape) and viewpoint-dependent relational properties (3D orientation relative to the viewer)–are perceptually represented and that properties of the proximal (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  28.  38
    Olfaction, valuation, and action: reorienting perception.Jason B. Castro & William P. Seeley - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    In the philosophy of perception, olfaction is the perennial problem child, presenting a range of difficulties to those seeking to define its proper referents, and its phenomenological content. Here, we argue that many of these difficulties can be resolved by recognizing the object-like representation of odors in the brain, and by postulating that the basic objects of olfaction are best defined by their biological value to the organism, rather than physico-chemical dimensions of stimuli. Building on this organism-centered account, we speculate (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  29. Connecting vision with the world: Tracking the missing link.Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 2001 - In Joao Branquinho (ed.), The Foundations of Cognitive Science. Oxford: Clarendon Press. pp. 183.
    You might reasonably surmise from the title of this paper that I will be discussing a theory of vision. After all, what is a theory of vision but a theory of how the world is connected to our visual representations? Theories of visual perception universally attempt to give an account of how a proximal stimulus (presumably a pattern impinging on the retina) can lead to a rich representation of a three dimensional world and thence to either the recognition of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  30.  18
    Viewpoint Invariance of Eye Size Illusion Caused by Eyeshadow.Hiroyuki Muto, Mayu Ide, Akitoshi Tomita & Kazunori Morikawa - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Previous research found that application of eyeshadow on the upper eyelids induces overestimation of eye size. The present study examined whether this eyeshadow illusion is dependent on or independent of viewpoint. We created a three-dimensional model of a female face and manipulated the presence/absence of eyeshadow and face orientation around the axis of yaw (Experiment 1) or pitch (Experiment 2) rotation. Using the staircase method, we measured perceived eye size for each face stimulus. Results showed that the eyeshadow illusion (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  19
    A kinematic study on (un)intentional imitation in bottlenose dolphins.Luisa Sartori, Maria Bulgheroni, Raffaella Tizzi & Umberto Castiello - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9:144694.
    The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of observing other’s movements on subsequent performance in bottlenose dolphins. The imitative ability of non-human animals has intrigued a number of researchers. So far, however, studies in dolphins have been confined to intentional imitation concerned with the explicit request to imitate other agents. In the absence of instruction to imitate, do dolphins (un)intentionally replicate other’s movement features? To test this, dolphins were filmed while reaching and touching a stimulus (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32. Biological motion: An exercise in bottom-up vs. top-down processing.Basileios Kroustallis - 2004 - Journal of Mind and Behavior 25 (1):57-74.
    Biological motion is the phenomenon of recognizing a human form out of moving point-light dots, where both bottom–up and top–down processing mechanisms have been reported. This study reviews available psychological and neuroscientific evidence, and it assesses attempts either to assimilate biological motion to other structure-from-motion cases or to include biological motion into a visual “social cognition” subsystem . While neither theoretical option seems to accommodate all relevant psychological results, the study proposes that biological motion may be an object recognition task, (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  13
    Children Only 3 Years Old Can Succeed at Conditional “If, Then” Reasoning, Much Earlier Than Anyone Had Thought Possible.Daphne S. Ling, Cole D. Wong & Adele Diamond - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    That conditional, if-then reasoning does not emerge until 4–5 years has long been accepted. Here we show that children barely 3 years old can do conditional reasoning. All that was needed was a superficial change to the stimuli: When color was a property of the shapes rather than of the background, 3-year-olds could succeed. Three-year-olds do not seem to use color to inform them which shape is correct unless color is a property of the shapes themselves. While CD requires integrating (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34. Some Contextual Reflections on 'Purpose in the Living World?'.Bruce C. Wearne - 2011 - Philosophia Reformata 76 (1):84-102.
    Jacob Klapwijk’s book Purpose in the Living World? (Cambridge 2998) is examined with special attention given to the scholarly background from out of which it emerges as a significant contribution to reformational philosophical reflection. As an initial step to clarify some important issues raised by Klapwijk’s critical comments about Dooyeweerd’s “essentialist” concept of species, the article probes facets of the way Jan Lever incorporated reformational philosophical concepts into his biological theory and considers the 1959 review written by Herman Dooyeweerd of (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35.  99
    Framework of Consciousness from Semblance of Activity at Functionally LINKed Postsynaptic Membranes.Kunjumon I. Vadakkan - 2010 - Frontiers in Psychology 1.
    Consciousness is seen as a difficult “binding” problem. Binding, a process where different sensations evoked by an item are associated in the nervous system, can be viewed as a process similar to associative learning. Several reports that consciousness is associated with some form of memory imply that different forms of memories have a common feature contributing to consciousness. Based on a proposed synaptic mechanism capable of explaining different forms of memory, we developed a framework for consciousness. It is based on (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  36.  32
    Reconsidering the Mind-Wandering Reader: Predictive Processing, Probability Designs, and Enculturation.Regina E. Fabry & Karin Kukkonen - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Studies on mind-wandering frequently use reading as an experimental task. In these studies, reading is conceived as a cognitive process that potentially offers a contrast to mind-wandering, because it seems to be task-related, goal-directed and stimulus-dependent. More recent work attempts to avoid the dichotomy of successful cognitive processes and processes of mind-wandering found in earlier studies. We approach the issue from the perspective that texts provoke modes of cognitive involvement different from information processing and recall account that underlies many (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  37. Framework of consciousness from semblance of activity at functionally LINKed postsynaptic membranes.Kunjumon Vadakkan - 2010 - Frontiers in Consciousness Research 1 (1):1-12.
    Consciousness is seen as a difficult “binding” problem. Binding, a process where different sensations evoked by an item are associated in the nervous system, can be viewed as a process similar to associative learning. Several reports that consciousness is associated with some form of memory imply that different forms of memories have a common feature contributing to consciousness. Based on a proposed synaptic mechanism capable of explaining different forms of memory, we developed a framework for consciousness. It is based on (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  81
    Two Routes to Expertise in Mental Rotation.Alexander Provost, Blake Johnson, Frini Karayanidis, Scott D. Brown & Andrew Heathcote - 2013 - Cognitive Science 37 (7):1321-1342.
    The ability to imagine objects undergoing rotation (mental rotation) improves markedly with practice, but an explanation of this plasticity remains controversial. Some researchers propose that practice speeds up the rate of a general-purpose rotation algorithm. Others maintain that performance improvements arise through the adoption of a new cognitive strategy—repeated exposure leads to rapid retrieval from memory of the required response to familiar mental rotation stimuli. In two experiments we provide support for an integrated explanation of practice effects in mental rotation (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  12
    High frequency force generation in outer hair cells from the mammalian ear.Matthew Holley - 1991 - Bioessays 13 (3):115-120.
    Mammalian outer hair cells generate mechanical forces at acoustic frequencies and can thus amplify the sound stimulus within the inner ear. The mechanism of force generation depends upon the plasma membrane potential but not upon either calcium or ATP. Forces are generated in the lateral cortex along the full length of the cell. The cortex includes a two‐dimensional cytoskeletal lattice composed of circumferential filaments 6–7 nm thick that are cross‐linked by filaments 3–4 nm thick and 40–60 nm long. The (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  5
    Visual Search in 3D: Effects of Monoscopic and Stereoscopic Cues to Depth on the Validity of Feature Integration Theory and Perceptual Load Theory.Ciara M. Greene, John Broughan, Anthony Hanlon, Seán Keane, Sophia Hanrahan, Stephen Kerr & Brendan Rooney - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Previous research has successfully used feature integration theory to operationalise the predictions of Perceptual Load Theory, while simultaneously testing the predictions of both models. Building on this work, we test the extent to which these models hold up in a 3D world. In two experiments, participants responded to a target stimulus within an array of shapes whose apparent depth was manipulated using a combination of monoscopic and stereoscopic cues. The search task was designed to test the predictions of feature (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  22
    A Penetrating Question in the History of Ideas: Space, Dimensionality and Impenetrability in the Thought of Avicenna.Jon Mcginnis - 2006 - Arabic Sciences and Philosophy 16 (1):47.
    Avicenna's discussion of space is found in his comments on Aristotle's account of place. Aristotle identified four candidates for place: a body's matter, form, the occupied space, or the limits of the containing body, and opted for the last. Neoplatonic commentators argued contra Aristotle that a thing's place is the space it occupied. Space for these Neoplatonists is something possessing dimensions and distinct from any body that occupies it, even if never devoid of body. Avicenna argues that this Neoplatonic notion (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  42.  16
    Dimensionality and measurement invariance of the italian version of the eortc qlq-c30 in postoperative lung cancer patients.Chiara Marzorati, Dario Monzani, Ketti Mazzocco, Francesca Pavan, Massimo Monturano & Gabriella Pravettoni - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43. The three-dimensionality of color: An evolutionary accommodation to an enduring property of the world.R. N. Shepard - 1992 - In Jerome Barkow, Leda Cosmides & John Tooby (eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford University Press. pp. 495--532.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  44.  37
    Comment: Language and Dimensionality in Appraisal Theory.Ronald de Sousa - 2013 - Emotion Review 5 (2):171-175.
    The proliferation of dimensions of appraisal is both welcome and worrying. The preoccupation with sorting out causes may be somewhat otiose. And the ubiquity of emotions in levels of processing raises intriguing problems about the role of language in identifying and triggering emotions and appraisals.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  45. The Exposure of Grace: Dimensionality in Late Heidegger.Andrew J. Mitchell - 2010 - Research in Phenomenology 40 (3):309-330.
    Heidegger's reflections on grace culminate in the years 1949-54 where grace names a figure for the ineluctable exposure of existence. Heidegger rethinks the relationship between what exists and the world in which it is found as one that is always open to grace. For Heidegger, this world is what he terms the “dimension” between earth and sky. The relationship is only possible where existence is no longer construed as a self-contained presence but instead is thought as something between presence and (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  46. Phenomenology and the multi-dimensionality of the body.Erol Copelj & Jack Alan Reynolds - 2022 - In Francois-Xavier de Vaujany, Jeremy Aroles & Mar Perezts (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Phenomenologies and Organisation Studies. pp. 123-145.
    The modern era has witnessed an extraordinary and unprecedented growth in our empirical knowledge regarding the human body. This raises the question: what, if anything, can phenomenology teach us about the body that the empirical sciences cannot? Whereas common sense and empirical sciences begin from the body as straightforwardly and obviously given and go on from there to think about what this thing is, what it is made up of, and how it originated, phenomenology steps back from the straightforward fact (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. An Assessment of the Dimensionality and Factorial Structure of the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale.Kenneth Drinkwater, Andrew Denovan, Neil Dagnall & Andrew Parker - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
  48.  35
    A penetrating question in the history of ideas: Space, dimensionality and interpenetration in the thought of avicenna.Jon Mcginnis - 2006 - Arabic Sciences and Philosophy 16 (1):47-69.
    Avicenna's discussion of space is found in his comments on Aristotle's account of place. Aristotle identified four candidates for place: a body's matter, form, the occupied space, or the limits of the containing body, and opted for the last. Neoplatonic commentators argued contra Aristotle that a thing's place is the space it occupied. Space for these Neoplatonists is something possessing dimensions and distinct from any body that occupies it, even if never devoid of body. Avicenna argues that this Neoplatonic notion (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  49. Marking the Perception–Cognition Boundary: The Criterion of Stimulus-Dependence.Jacob Beck - 2018 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 96 (2):319-334.
    Philosophy, scientific psychology, and common sense all distinguish perception from cognition. While there is little agreement about how the perception–cognition boundary ought to be drawn, one prominent idea is that perceptual states are dependent on a stimulus, or stimulus-dependent, in a way that cognitive states are not. This paper seeks to develop this idea in a way that can accommodate two apparent counterexamples: hallucinations, which are prima facie perceptual yet stimulus-independent; and demonstrative thoughts, which are prima facie (...)
    Direct download (11 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  50.  5
    A Novel Efficient Feature Dimensionality Reduction Method and Its Application in Engineering.Zhun Cheng & Zhixiong Lu - 2018 - Complexity 2018:1-14.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
1 — 50 / 1000